Goal-Setting in the Career Management Process: An Identity
Theory Perspective
Lindsey M. Greco
Oklahoma State University
Maria L. Kraimer
University of Iowa
A common and important feature within models of career management is the career goal, yet relatively
little is known about the factors influencing career goals and when and how career goal setting occurs.
Drawing from Ashforth’s (2001) model of role transitions we propose and test a model wherein
mentoring experiences of early career professionals relate to short- and long-term career goals through
professional identification. Using survey data collected at three points in time from 312 early career
professionals, we find that psychosocial mentoring, but not career mentoring, positively relates to
professional identification. For short-term goal outcomes, professional identification positively relates to
extrinsic goals, intrinsic goals, and goals that are high quality (i.e., specific, difficult, to which one is
committed). For long-term goal outcomes, professional identification positively relates to extrinsic and
intrinsic goals, but not to goal quality. Instead, in the long-term goal model, psychosocial mentoring is
directly related to goal quality. The theoretical and practical implications of this study for professional
identification, career goals, and how mentors can facilitate career goals are discussed.
Keywords: careers, goal-setting, mentoring, professional identification
Supplemental materials: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/apl0000424.supp
Employees are more actively involved in and responsible for the
management of their careers now than they have been in the past.
According to the boundaryless and protean careers perspectives,
the responsibility for charting and navigating one’s career is placed
firmly in the hands of the individual as opposed to his or her
employing organization (e.g., Arthur & Rousseau, 1996; Eby,
Butts, & Lockwood, 2003). Individual career planning and man-
agement processes are outlined in various career management
models including the widely cited frameworks of Greenhaus
(1987); London (1983), and Gould (1979). A common and impor-
tant feature within each model is the career goal, defined as any
desired career outcome, such as promotion, salary increase, or skill
acquisition which individuals wish to attain (Greenhaus, 1987;
Seibert, Kraimer, Holtom, & Pierotti, 2013). Specifically, in
Greenhaus’s (1987) model of career management, goal setting
facilitates the development and implementation of a career strat-
egy, which produces progress toward stated goals. In London’s
(1983) theory of career motivation, career insight is defined as the
clarity of an individual’s career goals, and setting and trying to
accomplish career goals is part of career motivation. Finally,
Gould’s (1979) model of career planning suggests that planning
career goals leads to enactment and attainment of career goals.
However, as Greenhaus, Callanan, and Godshalk (2010) note
“although many writers on career management discuss the virtues
of goal setting, there is little research in the area of career goals”
(p. 54). That is, within the various career management models and
related research, most work begins with the assumption individuals
already have career goals—the literature then focuses on career
strategies to achieve said goals. In the few studies that have
examined individual or personal career goals, they are positioned
as antecedents of work attitudes such as job satisfaction and
well-being (e.g., Brunstein, 1993; Brunstein, Schultheiss, & Gräss-
mann, 1998; Maier & Brunstein, 2001; Roberson, 1989, 1990), or
career goals are addressed in a tangential fashion (e.g., assessing
distance from career goals, goal importance, goal progress; Abele
& Spurk, 2009; Dik, Sargent, & Steger, 2008; Maier & Brunstein,
2001; Noe, 1996; Noe, Noe, & Bachhuber, 1990). Overall, re-
search and theory have not addressed factors influencing the
creation of career goals and when and how career goal setting
occurs (Seo, Barrett, & Bartunek, 2004).
It is important to understand the factors that influence career
goals for three main reasons. First, in the era of the boundaryless
or protean careers, employees cannot count on organizationally
imposed career goals to manage their careers, so understanding
factors that influence goal setting and the content of career goals
This article was published Online First June 6, 2019.
Lindsey M. Greco, Department of Management, Spears School of Busi-
ness, Oklahoma State University; Maria L. Kraimer, Department of Man-
agement and Organizations, Tippie College of Business, University of
Iowa.
Maria L. Kraimer is now at the School of Management and Labor
Relations at Rutgers University.
This article was presented at the 2018 Academy of Management Annual
Meeting. We thank Scott Seibert, Ernest O’Boyle, and Eean Crawford for
their helpful comments on an early version of this article.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Lindsey
M. Greco, Department of Management, Spears School of Business, Okla-
homa State University, 229 Business Building, Stillwater, OK 74078.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Journal of Applied Psychology
© 2019 American Psychological Association 2020, Vol. 105, No. 1, 40–57
0021-9010/20/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/apl0000424
40
can provide insight into themes within this new career manage-
ment process. In particular, understanding the content of career
goals offers a unique lens into how individuals conceptualize their
future work selves (e.g., Markus, 1983; Markus & Nurius, 1986)
and subsequently manage their careers. Second, because career
management models assume that career goals, like other work
goals, will direct an individual’s attention, time, and energy, career
goals provide a crucial organizing standard that guide career-
related decisions by motivating or limiting choices about how to
achieve desired career outcomes (King, 2004). For example, goals
can determine an employee’s search for feedback and information
seeking as different types of goals change the kind of information
in the environment that the individual perceives and attends to
(Ashford & Cummings, 1983). Therefore, understanding the cre-
ation and content of career goals can provide important informa-
tion for those early in their careers in establishing a career trajec-
tory. Third, goals and goal attainment are important characteristics
in theories of job-related attitudes; goals are expected to positively
relate to job satisfaction, organizational commitment, and organi-
zational identification to the extent that the job promotes the
attainment of valued goals (Allen & Meyer, 1997; Ashforth &
Mael, 1989; Locke, 1976; Mael & Ashforth, 1992). Thus, infor-
mation about the formation and content of individual goals can be
valuable in managing job-related affect. In sum, without knowing
the factors that may influence an individual’s career goals, our
understanding of the outcomes of those goals on the career man-
agement process is incomplete.
The present study uses an identity theory lens to examine the
creation of personal, career related goals in early career pro-
fessionals. Based on self-verification theory and Ashforth’s
(2001) model of role transitions (reviewed below), we examine
a mediated model in which mentoring experiences relate to
different types of career goals, through professional identifica-
tion. Based on the findings from a qualitative pilot study, we
examine career goals in terms of goal content, either intrinsic or
extrinsic, and goal quality, the extent to which one’s goal is
difficult and specific and one is committed to it. We test the
mediated model using a sample of graduate students preparing
for professional careers.
This study contributes to the literature on identity formation and
career management by examining the development of professional
identification and career goals during the role transition process.
Understanding how identification relates to the career goals early
career professionals set for themselves can be used as a basis for
understanding career aspirations and trajectories for professional
workers. This study also contributes to goal setting theory by
examining the content and characteristics of goals in the career
context. Career goal setting is often done absent formal goal
setting mechanisms, and this context has received sparse attention
in the both goal setting and careers literatures. We expand this
research by assessing both the content and quality of personal
career goals. A final contribution of the study is to the mentoring
and developmental relationships literature. Responding to calls to
explicitly define when and why mentoring is associated with
particular facets of socialization (Allen, Eby, Chao, & Bauer,
2017), we recognize that career mentoring is similar to serial
tactics and psychosocial mentoring is similar to investiture tactics.
We also identify a possible explanation, specific content and
quality of career goals, for why mentoring positively relates to
protégée career outcomes.
An Identity Theory Perspective of Career Goals
From the perspective of boundaryless (e.g., Arthur & Rousseau,
1996) and protean (e.g., Mirvis & Hall, 1996) careers, employees,
rather than the organization, are responsible for charting their own
career trajectories. However, the possible trajectories for individ-
uals’ careers are practically unlimited, with no single template
setting the standard for a particular career path. In place of an
organization providing some type of structure for career advance-
ment, workers, instead, may rely on identification with career-
related groups to help define and create career goals within the
career management process. Identification with a relevant group
can replace institutionalized career structures and provide a com-
pass for an individual beyond the walls of an employing organi-
zation (Fugate, Kinicki, & Ashforth, 2004).
One theoretical perspective that is particularly relevant to un-
derstanding how professionals develop career goals is Ashforth’s
(2001) model of role transitions and related identification process.
Ashforth’s (2001) model incorporates ideas from both social iden-
tification theory (Tajfel & Turner, 1979) and identity theory
(Stryker & Serpe, 1994). Based on identity theory, he defines role
identities as socially constructed definitions of who a role occupant
is; such role identities have the purpose of anchoring self-
conceptions in social domains. Drawing from social identification
theory, Ashforth (2001) then defines role identification as a spe-
cific form of social identification which occurs if and when an
individual comes to define him/herself in terms of the role identity.
The higher the level of role identification, the more likely that one
will internalize the role identity as a partial definition of self, and
the more likely one will be to faithfully enact that identity (Ash-
forth, 2001). Both of these identity processes are relevant to goal
setting because enacting the role identity can be done by partici-
pating in behavior that “reflects a meld of institutionalized expec-
tations and idiosyncratic refinements” (Ashforth, 2001; p. 222).
One way to enact an identity, then, is to set personal goals that are
informed by and consistent with others in the referent group.
In the current study, we examine the role of mentors from the
referent group (i.e., the profession) and identification with that
group (i.e., professional identification) to career goal-setting
among early career professionals. Professional identification refers
to the extent that a professional employee experiences a perceived
oneness or bond with his or her profession (Hekman, Bigley,
Steensma, & Hereford, 2009a). Professional workers, such as
nurses, doctors, lawyers, and academics, are an ideal population to
study because they are defined less by where they work and more
by what they do (Pratt, Rockmann, & Kaufmann, 2006). The
career goals of professionals tend to be based on personal expec-
tations and examination of what is of fundamental import in one’s
life, such as professional norms and values, rather than organiza-
tionally assigned initiatives (Seo et al., 2004). Further, Cantor and
Zirkel (1990) stated that individuals devote considerable energy to
the creation of meaningful goals mostly during transitional stages
of life. For professionals, graduate school is a key transitional
stage into professional life because it provides systematic training
and socialization into that role (Austin & McDaniels, 2006; Baker
& Pifer, 2011; Howskins & Ewens, 1999; Price, 2009). This
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
41
MENTORING, IDENTIFICATION, AND CAREER GOALS
presents a unique application of Ashforth’s (2001) model of role
transitions as it relates to professional identification and career
goal setting as the first stage of the career management process.
In Ashforth’s (2001) model of role transitions, he expands on
the importance of transitional phases in goal setting by describing
the transition process as the assimilation of professional goals and
individual goals. During this transitional stage, mentors are likely
to play a key role in the development of professional identification
and possible career goals. This is because receipt of mentoring
may help newcomers satisfy two key psychological motives. Spe-
cifically, Ashforth (2001) proposes that role entry arouses psycho-
logical motives, including the need for control and the need for
belonging, which a newcomer will seek to fulfill in the context of
the role. The need for control is defined as a need to “master and
to exercise influence over subjectively important domains” (Ash-
forth, 2001, p. 67) and is associated with behaviors such as
information seeking, feedback seeking, proactive behavior, and
self-management. The need for belonging is the desire for attach-
ment with others or a sense of belonging with a larger group and
leads members to assume they share certain goals, values, and
commitment to the collective.
1
According to Ashforth (2001), the
more that those two motives are met, the more likely a newcomer
is to internalize the role, and the greater the identification with the
role. In turn, identification leads the newcomer to faithfully enact
the role identity, which results, in the current model, in establish-
ing identity related goals. We test these core propositions from
Ashforth’s (2001) role transitions theory by testing a model in
which career and psychosocial mentoring are hypothesized to be
positively related to professional identification, which, in turn is
related to the content (extrinsic and intrinsic) and quality (goal
difficulty, specificity, and commitment) of personal career goals
among graduate school students (see Figure 1).
Mentoring and Professional Identification
Mentoring has a variety of different connotations, sometimes
referring to dyadic relationships, or the “classic” mentor relation-
ship between senior (mentor) and junior (protégé) colleagues (e.g.,
Chao, Walz, & Gardner, 1992; Mitchell, Eby, & Ragins, 2015),
and at other times referring to a web of developmental work
relationships including informal mentors, role models, and coaches
(e.g., Kram, 1988; Kram & Isabella, 1985). In the current study,
we allow for multiple mentors, thus, our definition of mentoring is
consistent with the broader web of developmental relationships,
which Kram (1988) describes as “the range of possible adult
working relationships that can provide developmental functions
for career development” (p. 4). It has long been recognized that
organizational insiders, or mentors, are an important aspect of the
newcomer socialization process (e.g., Austin, 2002; Bauer &
Green, 1998; Ellis, Nifadkar, Bauer, & Erdogan, 2017; Humberd
& Rouse, 2016). Socialization dynamics include multiple pro-
cesses that define how individuals learn the social knowledge and
skills necessary to assume a particular role such as orientation,
training, apprenticeship programs, mentoring, and general on-the-
job learning (Van Maanen, 1978; Van Maanen & Schein, 1979).
Van Maanen and Schein (1979) argue that organizations imple-
ment a variety of bipoloar tactics to integrate new employees. We
specifically integrate the two tactics of serial and investiture so-
cialization into our discussion of mentoring because they concern
the social or interpersonal aspects of the socialization process
(Jones, 1986) and thus are most relevant to the role of mentors.
Below we explain how serial socialization relates to Ashforth’s
(2001) motive for control while investiture socialization relates to
Ashforth’s (2001) motive for belonging.
First, the serial (vs. disjunctive) tactic involves learning the new
job from a role model such as a mentor, supervisor, or more
experienced peer (vs. having no prior role incumbents to learn
from). Serial modes of socialization provide newcomers with
built-in guidelines to organize and make sense of their organiza-
tional situation (Van Maanen, 1978). As such, the serial tactic
corresponds to Ashforth’s (2001) motive for control for new role
entrants in that serial socialization by mentors helps early career
professionals master and exercise influence over their new roles.
Second, the investiture (vs. divestiture) tactic is the degree to
which newcomers receive positive (vs. negative) social support
from experienced members. Positive support affirms a newcomer’s
identity, capabilities, and attributes and results in more concrete
role orientations (Van Maanen & Schein, 1979) and perceptions of
fit (e.g., Cable & Parsons, 2001). This socialization tactic corre-
sponds to Ashforth’s (2001) motive for belonging in that investi-
ture socialization by mentors provides positive social support,
friendship, and opportunities to build significant interpersonal
relationships in their new roles. In sum, through serial and inves-
titure tactics, experienced members of the profession (i.e., men-
tors) may facilitate early career professionals’ adjustment by giv-
ing the newcomers needed advice and instructing them in how to
do their new jobs and by making them feel like they belong in the
profession (Allen, Eby, Poteet, Lentz, & Lima, 2004; Ashforth &
Saks, 1996; Ostroff & Kozlowski, 1992).
Relatedly, Kram (1985) outlined two primary mentor functions
provided by developmental relationships—career development
support and psychosocial support—both of which can contribute
to an individual’s growth and advancement (Kram, 1988). Both
career and psychosocial mentoring have been positively associated
with important career outcomes such as promotions (Dreher &
Ash, 1990; Wayne, Liden, Kraimer, & Graf, 1999), income (Dre-
her & Ash, 1990), intrinsic job satisfaction (Chao et al., 1992), and
career commitment (Allen et al., 2004). We propose that both
career and psychosocial mentoring received in developmental re-
lationships will relate to graduate students’ professional identifi-
cation.
Career mentoring. Career development support, or career
mentoring, enhances protégé advancement in an organization or in
their career and includes functions such as sponsorship, exposure
and visibility, coaching, protection, and providing challenging
assignments. This type of mentoring is representative of a serial
socialization tactic wherein mentors, acting as experienced mem-
bers of the profession, provide newcomers with clear guidelines
and structure that helps them to organize and make sense of their
1
Ashforth (2001) notes that the need for control is conceptually similar
to the need for autonomy (McClelland, 1985) and the need for belonging
is similar to the need for affiliation (McClelland, 1985). However, Mc-
Clelland’s needs are most often characterized as individual differences—
individuals have consistently high or low desires for each—whereas Ash-
forth’s (2001) psychological motives for control and belonging are variable
and role specific, therefore warranting different terminology to distinguish
the motives from the individual differences.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
42
GRECO AND KRAIMER
new role (Van Maanen & Schein, 1979). In addition, Kram (1985)
described helping the protégé develop a sense of professional
competence and achieving long-term career goals as a primary
career mentor function.
Identity in a new role is often tied to concerns about competence
(Kram, 1985). Ashforth (2001) proposes that one of the psycho-
logical needs that newcomers seek to satisfy when entering a new
role is a need for control. As mentioned previously, the need for
control is defined as a need for agency and mastery in one’s role
and includes concepts related to competence, autonomy, self-
determination, and power. The premise of this motive is that when
one has control over one’s role, this creates a sense of involvement
and responsibility and allows one to more fully internalize the new
role identity. Ashforth (2001) notes that this motive answers the
question of “how”—how do I enact the new role?
Efforts to clarify one’s identity in terms of how to perform
competently are facilitated by career mentoring. Newcomers to a
role fulfill the need for control by obtaining information and
feedback about role requisite knowledge, expectations, and values
from more experienced members. In the process of mastering the
role (i.e., feeling control), the newcomer learns and develops skills
and knowledge on how to behave in role-appropriate ways. Career
mentoring exemplifies control in that an experienced colleague,
through coaching, is providing information, feedback, and support
to help the newcomer better understand the values, expectations,
and appropriate behaviors of the graduate program and profession
more broadly. Through exposure to challenging assignments, ca-
reer mentoring provides a newcomer with experiences that help
him or her develop professional knowledge and skills (i.e., in-
crease professional competencies). Satisfying this control motive
is also consistent with themes from social identification theory as
a mentor can help a newcomer shape him or herself in ways
consistent with in-group classification (Tajfel, 1978). A sense of
control over a role identity engendered by coaching from a mentor
enables one to own the identity and to more fully adopt it as an
authentic expression of the self. As such, fulfilling this motive
answers the question of “how” the newcomer can engage with the
new role and experience the power and success of one’s mentors
as one’s own.
Hypothesis 1: Career mentoring positively predicts profes-
sional identification.
Psychosocial mentoring. Psychosocial mentoring addresses
interpersonal aspects of the relationship between mentor and pro-
tégé and includes functions such as serving as a role model and
providing counseling, friendship, and advice (Kram, 1985). Psy-
chosocial mentoring is consistent with investiture socialization
tactics wherein experienced organizational members act as role
models for new recruits and provide positive social support (Van
Maanen & Schein, 1979). Whereas career mentoring is expected to
fulfill motives for control, psychosocial mentoring is expected to
fulfill motives for belonging. The motive for belonging is defined
broadly as a desire for attachment with others in a new role as well
as the desire to be a part of a community that shares common
interests (Ashforth, 2001; Bowlby, 1988; Brewer, 1993). This
motive answers the question of “who”—who shares this identity
with me?
When transitioning into a new role, the belonging motive is tied
to positive value judgments about one’s self relative to a target
social group. Drawing from sociometer theory (Leary & Baumeis-
ter, 2000), Ashforth (2001) proposes that social inclusion and a
sense of belonging enhance self-esteem, leading to identity con-
struction consistent with the referent group (e.g., Vignoles, Rega-
lia, Manzi, Golledge, & Scabini, 2006). This corresponds with
social identity theory wherein perceived group membership arises
out of a social comparison process where individuals differentiate
between in-group members similar to the self and out-group mem-
bers who are different from the self as a way to enhance self-
esteem (Tajfel & Turner, 1985). Psychosocial mentoring is ex-
pected to positively relate to professional identification because
psychosocial mentoring fosters attachment, or belonging, with
senior colleagues in the profession, which develops the newcom-
er’s self-concept (Kram, 1985). Psychosocial mentoring can build
self-esteem and interpersonal belonging in several ways. Through
serving as a role model, the mentor sets a desirable example that
the newcomer identifies with as representative of the profession.
Accompanied with acceptance and confirmation from the mentor,
the newcomer sees an idealized self in the mentor that allows one
to have a positive appraisal of one’s own value and viability as a
member of the group to which one aspires to belong. The more
psychosocial mentoring one receives (i.e., acceptance and confir-
mation, friendship, and counseling), the more likely that individual
will understand and feel similar to others in the profession, which
Figure 1. Partially mediated model of mentoring to short-term career goals through professional identification.
p .05.
ⴱⴱ
p .01.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
43
MENTORING, IDENTIFICATION, AND CAREER GOALS
increases feelings of self-esteem and in-group belonging. As such,
fulfilling the belonging motive answers the question of “who” the
newcomer shares an identity with and represents a positive eval-
uation of the future self (Leary & Baumeister, 2000). Therefore,
higher levels of psychosocial mentoring will relate to higher levels
of professional identification.
Hypothesis 2: Psychosocial mentoring positively predicts pro-
fessional identification.
Professional Identification and Career Goals
For goal outcome variables, we focus on goal content (i.e.,
extrinsic and intrinsic) and goal quality based on the findings from
the pilot study (available as an online supplemental materials) and
as a reflection of social identification theory and the broader goal
setting literature. We also further differentiate between short- and
long-term goals based on goal setting theory. Latham and Seijts
(1999) along with Bandura and Schunk (1981) argue that a key
motivational component in goal setting relies on setting both
short-term (i.e., proximal) and long-term (i.e., distal) goals. Short-
term goals, sometimes referred to as subgoals, allow individuals to
reframe complex long-term goals into smaller, more attainable
steps that increase the chance for feedback and strategy adjustment
in pursuit of long-term goals (Latham & Seijts, 1999; Seijts &
Latham, 2001). Research shows that when learning and motiva-
tion, as opposed to just motivation, are required for goal attain-
ment, then both short- and long-term goals are necessary for goal
attainment (Bandura & Simon, 1977; Morgan, 1985; Stock &
Cervone, 1990). Although we believe that both short- and long-
term goals are important outcomes of the mentoring process, there
is no theoretical rationale supporting differential relationships be-
tween professional identification and short- versus long-term
goals. Therefore, although we test the proposed model separately
for short- and long-term goals, we develop our hypotheses refer-
ring simply to “goals.”
Goal content can be broadly categorized into extrinsic/intrinsic
categories (Deci & Ryan, 2000). Extrinsic goals are defined as the
extent to which the individual’s career goals include “extrinsically
motivating attributes such as visible success, status and influence
within the organization or society, and high financial rewards”
(Seibert et al., 2013, p. 171). Extrinsic goals have an outward
orientation (Williams, Hedberg, Cox, & Deci, 2000) that reveal a
concern with external signs of self-worth and interpersonal com-
parison with others (Kasser & Ryan, 1996). Intrinsic goals are
defined as goals that include intrinsically motivating features such
as “continually gaining new skills and knowledge, having inter-
esting and challenging work, and having the opportunity to do
work that impacts society” (Seibert et al., 2013, p. 171). Intrinsic
goals are consistent with actualizing and growth tendencies and are
expected to satisfy basic and inherent psychological needs for
relatedness, competence, and growth (Deci & Ryan, 1985). Such
goals are expected to be inherently satisfying or valuable to an
individual and are not reliant on the contingent evaluation of others
(Kasser & Ryan, 1996).
Importantly, Deci and Ryan (2000) note that the content (ex-
trinsic vs. intrinsic) of goals is distinct from the motives for goals,
which are related to the reasons why people are pursuing the
particular goal. The authors argue that motivations for pursuing a
goal can range from being self-determined with little to no external
pressure, to completely controlled by external pressures. Further,
intrinsic or extrinsic goal content is not necessarily related to a
particular goal motive (self-determined vs. controlled). For exam-
ple, it is possible that early career academics may set an extrinsic
goal to get a job at a prestigious university because they personally
value the resources and opportunities associated with prestigious
universities (self-determined motive) or because they feel pressure
from graduate advisors to pursue a research career at a prestigious
university (controlled motive). Consistent with previous work
(e.g., Seibert et al., 2013), we focus only on the content of the goal
itself, rather than the potential motivation behind the goal, treating
intrinsic and extrinsic career goals as distinct, theoretically orthog-
onal constructs.
Goal content-extrinsic. The content of extrinsic goals in-
cludes status and financial outcomes, those that primarily entail
social recognition or approval of others or material rewards
(Kasser & Ryan, 1996; Seibert et al., 2013). The high public status
of the professions is unquestioned in nearly all data on occupa-
tional prestige (Abbott, 1981, 2014; Featherman & Hauser, 1976;
Nakao & Treas, 1994). The main argument is that wealth and
status are universally and highly valued in any society, so powerful
occupations are highly regarded by all individuals (Treiman,
2013). Studies have found that business and law students identified
with their chosen profession because the profession represented
the ability to have the salary and lifestyle associated with higher
earnings (Schleef, 2000). Other studies have shown that students
entering business and law professions identified with their chosen
occupation, not because they had high perceived self-aptitude, but
because the profession represented the ability to have the prestige
(Azizzadeh et al., 2003; Schleef, 2000) and social status (Gran-
field, 1992) associated with the profession. Thus, external markers
such as income and status are a crucial basis for authenticating
professional membership.
Self-verification theory can explain why those who identify
more with the profession are more likely to set goals with extrinsic
content. According to self-verification theory, people desire veri-
fication of their core self-views (Swann, 1983; Swann & Read,
1981
). Motivated by desires for consistency and stability, individ-
uals
strive to maintain consistent self-views and chronically rein-
force them through a variety of social processes. Specifically,
individuals prefer others to see them as they see themselves
(Swann, Stein-Seroussi, & Giesler, 1992) and seek out interactions
with others that reinforce their self-concept. The extent to which
people see professional identity as a significant part of their
self-conception is reflected by how much they identify with the
group (Tajfel & Turner, 1979). If early career professionals have
high levels of identification with the profession, they regard this
profession-based self-view as an indispensable part of their self.
To verify their self-view as professional group members, early
career professionals may set extrinsic goals consistent with char-
acteristics of the profession, such as extrinsic signs of status and
wealth. External goals represent “identity cues” that are highly
identifiable signs and symbols of the profession (e.g., Pratt &
Rafaeli, 1997). By setting goals that represent acquisition of signs
and symbols of who they are as professional members, extrinsic
goals allow early career professionals to verify their own sense of
self.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
44
GRECO AND KRAIMER
Hypothesis 3: Professional identification positively predicts
extrinsic goals.
Goal content-intrinsic. Individuals with intrinsic career goals
seek opportunities to learn and grow, they aspire to gain knowl-
edge through challenging work. Kasser and Ryan (1996) conclude
that a defining characteristic of intrinsic goals is that they are
inherently valuable or satisfying, rather than being dependent on
the contingent evaluations of others.
Many professions have stated purposes that align with intrinsic
ideals. For example, medical schools emphasize the importance of
helping those in need, business schools emphasize the goal of
improving workers’ lives and the economy, and education schools
emphasize the goal of improving society. These ideals are widely
endorsed by society and likely play some role for professionals in
entering their chosen programs. Studies of law students indicate
that some students enter law school for altruistic reasons, such as
wanting to help people and improve society through social justice
(Granfield, 1992; Schleef, 2000). Further, most professional fields
offer intellectually stimulating work based on years of advanced
education, suggesting that professionals find some degree of chal-
lenging work personally fulfilling. Even if professional students do
not have intrinsic motivations prior to entering the profession, to
the extent that they internalize professional standards and norms
through identification with the role, they will develop intrinsic
goals that reflect professional standards of conduct as part of the
self-verification process. Through adopting the perspective that
altruistic and intrinsic goals are important for professionals, early
career professionals are able to see themselves as others see them
(Swann, 1983). High levels of professional identification will then
positively relate to individual intrinsic goals because the profes-
sional role presents an opportunity to do meaningful and challeng-
ing work that also has the potential to have a positive impact on
others.
Hypothesis 4: Professional identification positively predicts
intrinsic goals.
Goal quality. Goal quality is represented by goals that align
with the primary tenets of goal setting theory, that is, career goals
that are difficult, specific, and to which individuals are committed.
Specific and difficult goals and goal commitment are the primary
features of effective goals within goal setting theory; such goals
are better at directing energy and attention necessary for goal
attainment (Locke & Latham, 1990, 2002). Early career profes-
sionals who identify with the profession are likely to set high
quality goals as part of the self-verification process which, at its
core, is based on uncertainty reduction. Individuals like to feel that
their social world is “knowable and controllable” (Swann, 1983,p.
34) and are motivated to see to it that neither their self-conceptions
nor the appraisals of others in relation to their self change in any
drastic way. When early career professionals identify with the
profession, they are likely to have a clear idea of what constitutes
success for professional members and are likely to model their
career goals after clear indications of success as a way to verify
their status as a professional member. High quality goals are clear
and specific and thus reduce uncertainty associated with variation
in self-concepts related to the professional identity.
In terms of goal difficulty, individuals high in professional
identification should set goals that reflect the high standards that
are prototypical of professional communities, thus those goals are
likely to be difficult to attain. Difficult goals are also those which
may be most visible to others (e.g., publications in high status
journal) and, as such, have the potential to act as identity cues
which can communicate the identity to others and support self-
verification processes. Finally, goal commitment is created be-
cause social identification theory suggests that if one identifies
with the profession then he or she will be more committed to
actions that maintain belonging to the group and verify identity as
a group member. As a result, high levels of professional identifi-
cation should lead to commitment to goals shaped by the profes-
sion.
Hypothesis 5: Professional identification positively predicts
goal quality.
Both career mentoring and psychosocial mentoring are expected
to be positively related to extrinsic and intrinsic goal content and
goal quality through professional identification. Professional iden-
tification is a key mediating variable between mentoring and
career goals because it reflects the degree to which the professional
role is subjectively important to role occupants (Ashforth, 2001)
and reflects views that a primary mentor function is to help the
protégé develop a sense of professional competence and achieve
career goals (Kram, 1985). Mentoring experiences provide the
foundation for professional expectations (e.g., goals; Noe et al.,
1990), but the extent to which those expectations are translated
into personal goals depends on whether professional standards are
internalized through professional identification. In other words, the
more one internalizes mentoring ideals through professional iden-
tification, the more likely he or she is to have intrinsic and extrinsic
goals (consistent with professional standards) and goals of high
quality. Goals are the future enactment of professional identities
(Ashforth, 2001) developed from mentoring experiences (Tenen-
baum, Crosby, & Gliner, 2001).
Hypothesis 6: The relationship between (a) career mentoring
and (b) psychosocial mentoring and goal outcomes (i.e., in-
trinsic goals, extrinsic goals, goal quality) is mediated by
professional identification.
Method
Sample and Procedure
Survey data was collected from graduate students in profes-
sional programs in the United States as recorded in the University
of Iowa’s IRB# 201507760: Career Goals and Professional Iden-
tification: Goal Setting in the Role Transition Process. Schools
were contacted based on their conference division in order to target
universities with large and diverse graduate programs; universities
from the Big 10, Big 12, Pac 12, and SEC were included in our
initial contact efforts. Links to the online survey were distributed
through e-mail. There were three surveys in total, each separated
by approximately 4 weeks. The purpose of collecting data over
three surveys was to reduce systematic errors related to common
method variance (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee, & Podsakoff,
2003). Invitations to complete the first online survey were distrib-
uted in two ways. First, we identified graduate program coordina-
tors from university websites and emailed asking them to forward
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
45
MENTORING, IDENTIFICATION, AND CAREER GOALS
the link to the first online survey to their graduate students. In total,
we e-mailed 388 graduate coordinators. Second, when e-mail
addresses for students were available on university websites, we
e-mailed the students directly with a link to the first online survey;
we directly e-mailed 3,635 students.
Survey 1 contained measures for career and psychosocial men-
toring, potential control variables, and demographics. Survey 2,
sent 4 weeks later, contained measures of professional identifica-
tion, open-ended questions for short-term goals and scale items for
short-term goal content (i.e., extrinsic and intrinsic) and goal
quality (i.e., specific, difficult, commitment). Short-term goals
were defined as “occupational goals that you hope to achieve soon
after graduation.” Survey 3, sent another 4 weeks later, contained
open-ended questions for long-term goals and scale items for
long-term goal content (i.e., extrinsic and status) and goal quality
(i.e., specific, difficult, commitment). Long-term goals were de-
fined as “occupational goals that you hope to achieve one day in
your career.”
In total, 704 graduate students responded to Survey 1; however,
a significant number only filled out part of the survey. After
eliminating respondents who did not provide an e-mail address
(which was necessary to send the follow-up surveys) and those
with missing data on a majority of study variables, the final sample
size was 480 respondents to Survey 1. Because graduate coordi-
nators forwarded the survey link to an unknown number of grad-
uate students, it is not possible to calculate a response rate. Invi-
tations to complete the second and third surveys were distributed
directly to the 480 graduate students who responded to Survey 1.
In total, 343 students responded to Survey 2 (71.4% response rate
of Survey 1 respondents) and 331 students responded to Survey 3
(68.9% response rate of Survey 1 respondents). After removing
participants with missing data on focal variables, the final sample
size was 312 for testing the model using short-term goals (short-
term goal [STG] model) and 243 for testing the model using
long-term goals (long-term goal [LTG] model).
Respondents were enrolled in programs from 28 different uni-
versities. Approximately one quarter (23.6%) of respondents were
enrolled in Master’s programs (e.g., MA, MS, MLS), while the
remaining three quarters (76.4%) of respondents were enrolled in
PhD programs or equivalent (e.g., PhD, Ed.S, MD/PhD). The
respondents represented a large variety of program fields: The 17
fields were subsequently categorized into “hard” and “social”
sciences. Hard sciences included about half of respondents
(53.2%) from the following fields: engineering/computer science,
health/medicine, biological and physical sciences, agricultural or
animal sciences, architecture, mathematics, chemistry, and astron-
omy. Social sciences (46.8%) included respondents from the fol-
lowing fields: arts/humanities, business, communication, educa-
tion, government, law/public policy/criminal justice, psychology/
social science, and library science. Programs ranged from 1 to 8
years, the mean program length was 4.45 years. Respondents had
been enrolled in their programs from .23 to 8 years and the mean
time in the program was 2.08 years.
2
A majority of respondents
(86.7%) were on track to finish on time. Students had a variety of
financial support including teaching assistantships (49.5%), re-
search assistantships (53.8%), and/or grants (19.8%).
The age of respondents ranged from 21 to 63 years, with a mean
age of 28.14 years; 54.6% of respondents were female; 6.4%
indicated they were of Hispanic origin. A majority of the sample
was Caucasian (75.2%), and the remaining respondents identified
as Asian (16.2%), African American (2.9%), Indian (2.4%), and
other (3.3%). About one quarter (24.6%) of respondents were
international students. Approximately half of the respondents were
married or living with a committed partner (57%) and the majority
did not have children (88.9%).
Measures
All measures were self-reported and items were measured on a
5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree)to5
(strongly agree) unless otherwise noted.
Mentoring. Both career mentoring and psychosocial mentor-
ing scales were from the shortened version of Dreher and Ash
(1990) reported in Kraimer, Seibert, Wayne, Liden, and Bravo
(2011) and contained four items each. Responses range from 1 (not
at all)to5(to a very large extent). Example items for career
mentoring ask to what extent faculty advisor(s) have “given or
recommended you for challenging assignments that present oppor-
tunities to learn new skills” and “given or recommended you for
assignments that helped you meet new colleagues.” Items were
averaged to create a single scale score (␣⫽.90). Examples of
psychosocial support include items such as the extent to which
advisor(s) have “conveyed empathy for the concerns and feelings
you have discussed with him/her” and “encouraged you to talk
openly about anxiety, fears, or concerns you have that may detract
from your work.” Items were averaged to create a single scale
score (␣⫽.89).
Professional identification. Professional identification was
measured using five items from Hekman et al. (2009). Example
items are “In general, when someone praises my profession, it
feels like a personal compliment” and “My field’s successes are
my successes.” Items were averaged to create a single scale score
(␣⫽.74).
Goal content. The extrinsic content of goals was assessed
with eight items: five scale items from Seibert et al. (2013) edited
to fit the academic context (e.g., replacing “company” with
“field”) and three additional items. Example items from Seibert et
al. (2013) are “It is important to me to achieve financial success in
my career” and “It is important for me to be seen by others as a
success in my career.” The three additional items were: “A high
income is one of my career goals”; “One’s success in this career
can be judged by the amount of money one makes”; and “Rank and
status are important to me in my career.”
Intrinsic goals were measured with seven items: five scale items
from Seibert et al. (2013) and two additional items. Example items
from Seibert et al. (2013) are “I want to have a positive impact on
other people or social problems through my work” and “It is
important for me to continue to learn and grow over the course of
my career.” The two additional items were “I want to do work that
is important and meaningful” and “I want to have a positive impact
on organizations and society through my work.” Extrinsic and
intrinsic goals were measured twice, once with respect to short-
2
As a robustness check for potential outliers in the sample, we also ran
our analyses removing six respondents who had been in their programs 6
or more years and indicated they were not on track to finish on time. The
analyses with this reduced sample did not substantively change results of
the hypothesis testing, thus, we retained all respondents in the analyses.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
46
GRECO AND KRAIMER
term goals in Survey 2 and once for long-term goals in Survey 3.
Instructions on the respective surveys asked respondents to think
about their short- or long-term goals and asked them to rate the
extent they agreed with the items with reference to their goals.
Items were averaged to create a single scale score for short-term
extrinsic (␣⫽.82), short-term intrinsic (␣⫽.78), long-term
extrinsic (␣⫽.81), and long-term intrinsic (␣⫽.84) goals.
Goal quality. On Survey 2, respondents were asked to gen-
erate short-term goals. These goals were explained as goals that
one hoped to achieve soon after graduation, such as getting a
particular type of job or getting a job at a particular university/
institution. On Survey 3, respondents were asked to generate
long-term goals. These goals were explained as occupational goals
that one hopes to achieve someday in his or her career, or goals
representing more distant occupational aspirations. We asked re-
spondents to list two short-term goals (Survey 2) and two long-
term goals (Survey 3), as this was the average of the open-ended
request for self-reported goals from the Pilot Study (included as an
online supplement). Across the respondents, a total of 626 short-
term goals and 519 long-term goals were reported.
After each open-ended goal, respondents replied to statements
related to goal difficulty, goal specificity, and goal commitment.
Goal difficulty was measured with two items from Steers (1976).
The items were “This goal will require a great deal of effort from
me to complete” and “This goal is quite difficult to attain.” Goal
specificity was measured with two questions from Steers (1976);
the two items were “This goal is very clear and specific” and “I
have a clear sense of how to achieve this career goal.” Goal
commitment was measured using two items from Hollenbeck,
Klein, O’Leary, and Wright (1989): “I am strongly committed to
pursuing this goal” and “It would take a lot to make me abandon
this goal.”
Before creating scale scores for goal quality, two raters inde-
pendently coded all self-reported short- and long-term goal state-
ments in terms of professional relevance (1 relevant; 0 not
relevant). The two raters had a moderate level of agreement based
on guidelines from Altman (1999) and adapted from Landis and
Koch (1977), Cohen’s ␬⫽.58; any disagreements were resolved
through discussion. The purpose of this coding was to remove any
goals that were not professionally relevant when creating the goal
quality score. Of the short-term goals, 38 were deemed not rele-
vant (e.g., “To get a great job in Seattle, Washington”); of the
long-term goals, 20 were deemed not relevant (e.g., “I want to own
and operate my own airplane by age 40”). The dummy code for the
relevancy of the goal (1 or 0) was then multiplied by the average
of the goal difficulty, specificity, and commitment items for each
goal and all 0 values were subsequently marked as missing data in
the analysis. In this way, the score for goal quality excludes goals
that were not professionally relevant.
The relationship between the higher order goal quality construct
and the three first-order dimensions was assessed with a second-
order confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). A model was specified
in which first-order factors for difficulty, specificity, and commit-
ment was predicted by a higher-order goal quality construct;
because each first-order factor related to two distinct goals, errors
were correlated between items assessing the same goal. The STG
model demonstrated moderately good fit (
2
190.56; RMSEA
.10; CFI .86; SRMR .07) and each factor loaded onto the
hypothesized goal quality variable (
difficulty
.86, p .00;
specificity
.79, p .00;
commitment
.94, p .00). The fit of
the second-order model exhibited significantly better fit than a
single-factor model (⌬␹
2
10.84, df 3, p .05). Credé and
Harms (2015) recommend providing the average variance ex-
tracted (AVE) which summarizes the ability of the higher-order
factor to account for variance in the lower-order factors; the AVE
for the STG model was .75, which surpasses the recommended
value of .50 (Credé & Harms, 2015; Johnson, Rosen, Chang,
Djurdevic, & Taing, 2012; Johnson, Rosen, & Djurdjevic, 2011).
The LTG model demonstrated good fit (
2
126.62; RMSEA
.09; CFI .91; SRMR .06) with each first order factor signif-
icantly loading onto the goal quality variable (
difficulty
.45, p
.00;
specificity
.79, p .00;
commitment
.96, p .00). The fit
of the second-order model exhibited significantly better fit than a
single-factor model (⌬␹
2
19.20, df 3, p .05) and the AVE
for the second-order factor model was .58. Although some of the
fit indices are slightly below recommended cut-off points for
“acceptable fit,” the model comparisons, AVE scores, and corre-
lations among the dimensions (r ranges from .38 to .58 for short-
term goals and from .36 to .76 for long-term goals) suggest the
dimensions can be represented by a single overall construct, goal
quality. Both short-term (␣⫽.80) and long-term (␣⫽.77) goal
quality demonstrated adequate levels of reliability.
Control variables. We included two relevant control vari-
ables following recommended guidelines (Aguinis & Vanden-
berg, 2014; Becker, 2005; Bernerth & Aguinis, 2016). First, we
included whether expectations about the profession were met as
a control variable based on Ashforth’s (2001) model of identi-
fication. Ashforth (2001) positions “met expectations” as a
precursor to identity-related processes and subsequent identifi-
cation; the rationale is that newcomers have certain expecta-
tions about new roles, and when these expectations do not
match reality, they can experience reality shock which affects
subsequent identification processes (Ashforth, 2001; Major,
Kozlowski, Chao, & Gardner, 1995). We measured met expec-
tations with two items measured on Survey 1: “To what extent
have your expectations about the profession been met” and “All
in all, have your expectations with regard to the profession been
met” (␣⫽.89, Arnold & Feldman, 1982; Lee & Mowday,
1987). Second, we controlled for the length of time the student
had been in their graduate program because both career goals
and mentoring experiences are likely to differ significantly
between the beginning and end of a graduate student’s tenure
(e.g., Humberd & Rouse, 2016). We considered other control
variables such as the program field (hard vs. social science),
age, gender, and whether the respondent was an international
student, but these variables did not affect the results in a
meaningful way. Therefore, we include these variables in the
correlation table but not in further analyses.
Results
The means, standard deviations, and intercorrelations among con-
trol and study variables are shown in Table 1. Both career mentoring
(r .20) and psychosocial mentoring (r .30) are positively related
to professional identification. In turn, professional identification is
positively related to all short-term (r
extrinsic
.30; r
intrinsic
.24;
r
quality
.20) and long-term goal (r
extrinsic
.27; r
intrinsic
.26;
r
quality
.21) variables.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
47
MENTORING, IDENTIFICATION, AND CAREER GOALS
Confirmatory Factor Analysis
We first conducted a CFA, using Mplus 7.2, to assess the extent
to which scale items captured intended constructs. When con-
structs had more than six items as indicators we created parcels.
Given the large number of items relative to sample size in the
current study, parcels enabled us to maintain a better sample
size-to-parameter ratio and decreased the likelihood of identifica-
tion problems in the CFA (Williams & O’Boyle, 2008). Parcels
were created using the item-to-construct balance approach pre-
sented in Little, Cunningham, Shahar, and Widaman (2002) with
the exception of the parcels for the goal quality variable. Goal
quality is multidimensional and comprised of distinct subscales
(i.e., difficulty, specificity, commitment), so items from each sub-
scale were grouped into conceptually relevant parcels; this maxi-
mizes the internal consistency of each parcel for multidimensional
latent variables (Williams & O’Boyle, 2008).
For the hypothesized models, we performed two separate CFAs,
one using short-term goals (the STG model), and the other using
long-term goals (the LTG model). Each item or parcel was fit to its
relative factor. In the CFA for both models, a total of six constructs
were included in each analysis: career mentoring (four items),
psychosocial mentoring (four items), professional identification
(five items), short-/long-term goal content-extrinsic (three parcels
each), short-/long-term goal content-intrinsic (three parcels each),
short-/long-term goal quality (three parcels each). All factor load-
ings on the specified factors were significant, which indicates that
the items and parcels were acceptable indicators for the designated
latent variables.
For the STG model, the hypothesized six-factor model demon-
strated good fit (
2
374.15; RMSEA .05; CFI .94;
SRMR .05). A test of alternative models showed that the
hypothesized model had better fit than a five-factor model where
career mentoring and psychosocial mentoring were modeled as a
single “mentoring” variable (⌬␹
2
509.79, df 1, p .01), a
five-factor model where extrinsic and intrinsic goal content vari-
ables were combined into a single goal content variable (⌬␹
2
353.76, df 1, p .01), or a four-factor model where all goal
variables were combined into a single factor (⌬␹
2
406.49,
df 3, p .01).
For the LTG model, the hypothesized six-factor model demon-
strated good fit (
2
363.06; RMSEA .06; CFI .94;
SRMR .06). A test of alternative models showed that the
hypothesized model had better fit than a five-factor model where
career mentoring and psychosocial mentoring were modeled as a
single “mentoring” variable (⌬␹
2
436.58, df 1, p .01), a
five-factor model where extrinsic and intrinsic goal content vari-
ables were combined into a single goal content variable (⌬␹
2
251.87, df 1, p .01), or a four-factor model where all goal
variables were combined into a single factor (⌬␹
2
316.44,
df 3, p .01).
We also computed the AVE estimates for the 10 scales (Fornell
& Larcker, 1981); the AVE estimate is the average amount of
variation that a latent construct is able to explain in the observed
variable to which it is theoretically related (Farrell, 2010). For the
control variable, AVE was .80 for met expectations. For the model
variables, AVE values were .55 for career mentoring, .56 for
psychosocial mentoring, .39 for professional identification, .64 for
STG content–extrinsic, .59 for STG content–intrinsic, .31 for STG
quality, .62 for LTG content–extrinsic, .65 for LTG content–
intrinsic, and .32 for LTG quality. A purpose of calculating the
Table 1
Descriptive Statistics and Correlations for Study Variables
Variable MSDAVE1 2 3456789101112131415
1. Gender .45 .50
2. Age 28.14 5.81 .06
3. International
student .25 .43 .24
ⴱⴱ
.02
4. Program field .47 .50 .01 .16
ⴱⴱ
.00
5. Met expectations 3.81 .86 .89 .08 .02 .09 .01 .88
6. Time in program
(in years) 2.05 1.69 .06 .28
ⴱⴱ
.02 .01 .22
ⴱⴱ
7. Career
mentoring 3.13 1.09 .74 .08 .05 .09 .01 .22
ⴱⴱ
.03 .90
8. Psychosocial
mentoring 3.09 1.16 .75 .05 .02 .07 .10 .35
ⴱⴱ
.09 .49
ⴱⴱ
.89
9. Professional
identification 3.57 .72 .63 .22
ⴱⴱ
.06 .03 .02 .21
ⴱⴱ
.12
.20
ⴱⴱ
.30
ⴱⴱ
.74
10. STG content-
extrinsic 3.43 .67 .80 .04 .07 .15
.03 .06 .07 .13
.10 .30
ⴱⴱ
.82
11. STG content-
intrinsic 4.63 .37 .77 .16
ⴱⴱ
.02 .15
.09 .07 .04 .08 .12
.24
ⴱⴱ
.14
.78
12. STG quality 3.89 .85 .56 .06 .04 .01 .12
.22
ⴱⴱ
.01 .05 .08 .20
ⴱⴱ
.21
ⴱⴱ
.16
ⴱⴱ
.80
13. LTG content-
extrinsic 3.42 .64 .79 .01 .12 .23
ⴱⴱ
.01 .08 .04 .15
.14
.27
ⴱⴱ
.82
ⴱⴱ
.11 .10 .81
14. LTG content-
intrinsic 4.63 .43 .80 .11 .03 .20
ⴱⴱ
.00 .01 .04 .11 .06 .26
ⴱⴱ
.19
ⴱⴱ
.62
ⴱⴱ
.19
ⴱⴱ
.14
.84
15. LTG quality 3.88 .78 .57 .01 .03 .08 .12 .14
.09 .17
ⴱⴱ
.10 .21
ⴱⴱ
.27
ⴱⴱ
.19
ⴱⴱ
.40
ⴱⴱ
.22
ⴱⴱ
.30
ⴱⴱ
.77
Note. N ranges from 312 (STG model), 243 (LTG model); STG short-term goal; LTG long-term goal; AVE average variance extracted; values
in italics are alpha reliabilities. Gender is coded 0 female, 1 male; international student is coded 0 no, 1 yes; program field is coded 0 hard
sciences, 1 soft sciences. Variables 1–4 were potential control variables and are not included in model estimation.
p .05.
ⴱⴱ
p .01.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
48
GRECO AND KRAIMER
AVE is to establish discriminant validity in latent variables. Dis-
criminant validity is supported when the AVE of each construct is
greater than its shared variance (i.e., square of the correlation) with
any other construct (Fornell & Larcker, 1981) or, alternatively, the
square root of the AVE (AVE) is greater than the raw correla-
tion; these values are presented in Table 1. For all but one of the
bivariate relationships the AVE is greater than the correlation.
The correlation between STG content-extrinsic and LTG content-
extrinsic is higher than the AVE value; however, hypothesis
testing is done separately for STG and LTG models, so there is no
need to demonstrate discriminant validity between these variables.
Hypothesis Testing
After assessing model fit of the measurement model, we tested
the theoretical model using structural equation modeling (SEM;
Mplus 7.2) with maximum-likelihood estimation. Any missing
data was coded as such in the data file; this analysis assumes that
data are missing completely at random, uses pairwise deletion
(Muthen & Muthen, 2007), and computes standard errors for
parameter estimates based on the observed information matrix
(Kenward & Molenberghs, 1998). We evaluate the model fit and
hypotheses independently for both STG and LTG models.
Short-term goal model. The hypothesized mediated model
using short-term goals fit the data well (
2
512.94; RMSEA
.05; CFI .93; SRMR .06). The fit of the hypothesized STG
model was then compared with four alternative models as reported
in Table 2. The model comparisons were between the hypothesized
model and three partially mediated models that included direct
paths from the independent variables to the goal outcomes; each
detailed in Table 2. In models including paths from career men-
toring to goal outcomes (⌬␹
2
3.19, df 3, ns), psychosocial
mentoring to goal outcomes (⌬␹
2
5.98, df 3, ns), or both
mentoring to all outcomes (⌬␹
2
7.42, df 6, ns) there was no
significant difference in model fit. However, examination of the
individual paths showed a significant direct relationship between
psychosocial mentoring and the goal quality variable. Conse-
quently, we tested a final parsimonious model that included a path
from psychosocial mentoring to goal quality. Including this path
did significantly improve model fit from the hypothesized model
(⌬␹
2
4.81, df 1, p .05). We report and interpret findings
from the partially mediated model (Model 5 in Table 2).
The results of the STG model are presented in Figure 1. Hypothesis
1 predicted a positive relationship between career mentoring and
professional identification, which was not supported (␤⫽.09, p
.27). Hypothesis 2 predicted a positive relationship between psycho-
social mentoring and professional identification, which was supported
(␤⫽.26, p .01). Together, career and psychosocial mentoring
explained 17% of the variance (R
2
.17, p .01) in professional
identification. Hypotheses 3 and 4 predicted a positive relationship
between professional identification and both STG content-extrinsic
and STG content-intrinsic, respectively. Both hypotheses were sup-
ported (
Extrinsic
.27, p .01;
Intrinsic
.30, p .01). Finally,
Hypothesis 5 predicted that professional identification would be pos-
itively related to STG quality, which was also supported (␤⫽.40,
p .01).
Hypothesis 6 predicted that professional identification would
mediate the relationship between mentoring and career goals. The
indirect effects of the career and psychosocial mentoring on the
goal outcomes through professional identification are listed in
Table 3. None of the indirect effects from career mentoring to goal
outcome variables were significant, thus, Hypothesis 6a was not
supported. In contrast, the indirect effects of psychosocial mentor-
ing on STG content-extrinsic (.07), STG content-intrinsic (.08),
and STG quality (.10) were all significant (p .05), providing
support for Hypothesis 6b. Lastly, in the partially mediated model,
psychosocial mentoring (␤⫽.17, p .05) had a positive, direct
relation to STG quality. Variance explained for each of the goal
outcomes was: STG content-extrinsic (R
2
.07, p .05), STG
content-intrinsic (R
2
.09, p .05), and STG quality (R
2
.24,
p .01).
Long-term goal model. The hypothesized mediated model
using long-term goals fit the data well (
2
460.24; RMSEA
.05; CFI .93; SRMR .07). The fit of the hypothesized LTG
model was again compared to alternative path models as reported
in Table 2. In models including direct paths from career mentoring
to goal outcomes (⌬␹
2
9.35, df 3, p .05) and psychosocial
mentoring to goal outcomes (⌬␹
2
10.96, df 3, p .05) there
was significant improvement in model fit. Including paths from
Table 2
Structural Model Fit for Short- and Long-Term Goal Outcomes
Model
2
df RMSEA [90% CI] CFI SRMR ⌬␹
2
Short-term goal model
1. Fully mediated (hypothesized) 512.94 263 .05 [.04, .06] .93 .06
2. Partially mediated (career mentoring to all outcomes) 509.75 260 .05 [.04, .06] .93 .06 3.19
3. Partially mediated (psychosocial mentoring to all outcomes) 506.96 260 .05 [.04, .06] .93 .06 5.98
4. Partially mediated (both mentoring to all outcomes) 505.52 257 .05 [.04, .06] .93 .06 7.42
5. Partially mediated (psychosocial mentoring to goal quality) 508.13 262 .05 [.04, .06] .93 .06 4.81
Long-term goal model
1. Fully mediated (hypothesized) 460.24 263 .05 [.04, .06] .93 .07
2. Partially mediated (career mentoring to all outcomes) 450.89 260 .05 [.04, .06] .93 .06 9.35
3. Partially mediated (psychosocial mentoring to all outcomes) 449.28 260 .05 [.04, .06] .94 .06 10.96
4. Partially mediated (both mentoring to all outcomes) 446.61 257 .05 [.04, .06] .93 .06 13.63
5. Partially mediated (psychosocial mentoring to goal quality) 449.39 262 .05 [.04, .06] .94 .06 10.85
ⴱⴱ
Note. All models are compared to fully mediated model. RMSEA root-mean-square error of approximation; CI confidence interval; CFI
comparative fit index; SRMR standardized root-mean-square residual.
p .05.
ⴱⴱ
p .01.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
49
MENTORING, IDENTIFICATION, AND CAREER GOALS
both types of mentoring to goal outcomes also significantly im-
proved model fit from the hypothesized model (⌬␹
2
13.63,
df 6, p .05). Examination of the individual paths suggested
that the improvement in fit was attributable to the direct relation-
ship between psychosocial mentoring and the goal quality variable
in particular. Thus, we ran a more parsimonious fifth model, where
psychosocial mentoring had a direct path only to LTG quality. This
model had a significant improvement in model fit over the hypoth-
esized model (⌬␹
2
10.85, df 1, p .01). Therefore, we base
our interpretation for the LTG model on Model 5 (see Figure 2).
Hypothesis 1 predicted a positive relationship between career men-
toring and professional identification, which was not supported (␤⫽
.07, p .42) and Hypothesis 2 predicted a positive relationship
between psychosocial mentoring and professional identification,
which was supported (␤⫽.28, p .01). Together, career and
psychosocial mentoring explained 19% of the variance (R
2
.19, p
.01) in professional identification. Hypothesis 3 and 4 predicted a
positive relationship between professional identification and both
LTG content-extrinsic and LTG content-intrinsic, respectively. Both
hypotheses were supported (
Extrinsic
.23, p .01;
Intrinsic
.22,
p .01). Finally, Hypothesis 5 predicted that professional identifi-
cation would be positively related to LTG quality, which was not
supported by the data (␤⫽.19, p .05).
The retained partially mediated model included a direct path
from psychosocial mentoring to LTG quality. This direct path,
along with the indirect effects of all independent variables on the
goal outcomes through professional identification are listed in
Table 3. The indirect effects test whether professional identifica-
tion mediates the relationship between mentoring and the LTG
outcomes per Hypothesis 6. The indirect effects from career men-
toring to all goal outcome variables were not significant. Thus,
Hypothesis 6a was not supported. In contrast, the indirect effects
of psychosocial mentoring on LTG content-extrinsic (.06, p .05)
and LTG content-intrinsic (.06, p .05) through professional
identification were significant. However, the indirect effect from
psychosocial mentoring to LTG quality (.05) was not. Thus, Hy-
pothesis 6b was supported with respect to LTG content, but not
quality. Lastly, in the partially mediated model psychosocial men-
toring (␤⫽.31, p .01) had a positive, direct relation to LTG
quality. Variance explained for each of the goal outcomes was:
LTG content-extrinsic (R
2
.05, p .12), LTG content-intrinsic
(R
2
.05, p .12), and STG quality (R
2
.18, p .01).
In summary, the methodological differences between the STG and
LTG models were the inclusion of short-term versus long-term goals
as outcomes and sample size (n 312 vs. n 243), respectively. We
found that a partially mediated model with a direct path from psy-
chosocial mentoring to goal quality fit the data best for both STG and
LTG models. There was consistency across both models; the primary
difference in findings was that psychosocial mentoring had direct and
indirect effects with goal quality in the STG model whereas it was
directly, but not indirectly, related to goal quality in the LTG model.
Supplementary Analysis
Although the hypothesized model proposed independent and
direct effects for career and psychosocial mentoring, we performed
several supplementary analyses to probe potential interactive or
compensatory effects of mentoring on professional identification.
3
First, it is unlikely that either career or psychosocial mentoring
functions occur completely independently of one another. The
effect of one mentoring function could be dependent on another,
suggesting the presence of a mentoring “profile” including both
mentoring functions. To test this premise, we modeled interactive
effects of career and psychosocial mentoring on professional iden-
tification, but the interaction term was not significant in the STG
(␤⫽⫺.03, p .69) or LTG (␤⫽⫺.02, p .67) models. This
suggests that there is not a particular mentoring profile that pre-
dicts professional identification beyond the independent direct
effects.
Second, because it is possible that mentoring functions have
compensatory or complementary effects, we tested the effects of
the mentoring functions independently, running two separate anal-
3
We thank two anonymous reviewers for these suggestions.
Table 3
Direct and Indirect Effects From Mentoring Variables to Short- and Long-Term Goal Outcomes
IV Path
Standardized
path estimate 95% CI
Short-term goal model
Career mentoring Professional Identification STG Content-Extrinsic .02 [.02, .06]
Professional Identification STG Content-Intrinsic .03 [.02, .07]
Professional Identification STG Quality .03 [.02; .10]
Psychosocial mentoring Professional Identification STG Content-Extrinsic .07
[.02, .12]
Professional Identification STG Content-Intrinsic .08
[.02, .14]
Professional Identification STG Quality .10
[.03, .18]
LTG Quality .17
[.02; .32]
Long-term goal model
Career mentoring Professional Identification LTG Content-Extrinsic .02 [.02, .06]
Professional Identification LTG Content-Intrinsic .02 [.02, .05]
Professional Identification LTG Quality .01 [.02, .05]
Psychosocial mentoring Professional Identification LTG Content-Extrinsic .06
[.01, .12]
Professional Identification LTG Content-Intrinsic .06
[.01, .12]
Professional Identification LTG Quality .05 [.01, .12]
LTG Quality .31
[.13, .48]
Note.CI confidence interval; STG short-term goal; LTG long-term goal; direct paths are bolded.
p .05.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
50
GRECO AND KRAIMER
yses with each including only one type of mentoring. When
psychosocial mentoring is excluded from the path model, there is
a positive relationship between career mentoring and professional
identification in both the STG (␤⫽.21, p .01) and LTG (␤⫽
.20, p .01) models. The indirect effects of career mentoring on
the goal outcomes through professional identification are also
significant in the STG (
Extrinsic
.06, p .05;
Intrinsic
.06,
p .05;
Quality
.10, p .01) and LTG (
Extrinsic
.05, p
.05;
Intrinsic
.05, p .05;
Quality
.07, p .01) models. This
is a change from the full hypothesized model where the path from
career mentoring to professional identification was near zero and
nonsignificant and the indirect effects were also nonsignificant.
When career mentoring is excluded from the path model, there is
a positive relationship between psychosocial mentoring and pro-
fessional identification in both STG (␤⫽.29, p .01) and LTG
(␤⫽.32, p .01) models. The indirect effects of psychosocial
mentoring on the goal outcomes through professional identifica-
tion were also significant in the STG (
Extrinsic
.08, p .01;
Intrinsic
.09, p .01;
Quality
.05, p .05) and LTG
(
Extrinsic
.07, p .05;
Intrinsic
.07, p .05;
Quality
.11,
p .01) models. Taken together, career mentoring has a compen-
satory effect when psychosocial mentoring is not present, while
psychosocial mentoring subsumes both mentoring functions in the
prediction of professional identification.
Discussion
Career goals are advantageous because they enable a person to
direct his or her efforts in a relatively focused manner with a clear
plan of action. Despite the many models of career management
referencing goal-related concepts (i.e., Gould, 1979; Greenhaus,
1987; London, 1983), there has been little empirical research that
examines factors that predict the types of career goals individuals
set for themselves. Drawing from Ashforth’s (2001) model of role
transitions, we specifically examined how mentoring experiences
and professional identification related to the content and quality of
early career professionals’ career goals.
We found that psychosocial mentoring, but not career mentor-
ing, was positively related to professional identification in the
hypothesized model. Career mentoring, which includes functions
such as sponsorship, coaching, exposure, and visibility, was ex-
pected to fulfill the control motive (Ashforth, 2001) through help-
ing early career professionals develop their professional compe-
tence and shape themselves in ways consistent with in-group
classification (Tajfel, 1978). In contrast, psychosocial mentoring,
which is based on interpersonal functions such as providing friend-
ship and counseling (Kram, 1985), was expected to relate to
professional identification because social support from a mentor
can increase feelings of in-group belonging (Ashforth, 2001;
Turner, 1985). Considering that the identification process, at its
core, draws from inclusion in “in-groups,” our findings suggest
that early career professionals are more likely to consider them-
selves to be part of the professional in-group when they have
faculty mentors who engage with them on a personal level and
engender a sense of belonging, rather than mentors who focus on
their career-related success and fulfill motives related to control.
However, supplementary analysis showed that, when tested
independently, career mentoring positively related to professional
identification, suggesting possible compensatory effects of career
mentoring when psychosocial mentoring is absent. Although each
form of mentoring is not contingent on the other dimension, as
demonstrated by an insignificant interaction effect, the indepen-
dent effects of each type of mentoring are notable. Psychosocial
mentoring positively related to professional identification regard-
less of the presence of career mentoring in the model, whereas
career mentoring was only positively related to professional iden-
tification in the absence of psychosocial mentoring, suggesting that
career mentoring can compensate or “stand in” for a lack of
psychosocial mentoring support in relating to professional identi-
fication. To some extent, this speaks to the overlap of mentoring
functions; it is likely that psychosocial mentoring contains career-
related elements. For example, having dinner at a professional
conference with a mentor and coauthors may satisfy both psycho-
social and career functions. While a protégé may consider this
dinner to be ‘friendly’ and representative of psychosocial support,
the interaction likely confers career related benefits, which could
explain the lack of significance for career mentoring as an inde-
pendent predictor in the full model.
In the STG model, professional identification positively related
to all three goal outcome variables. Thus, for early career profes-
sionals contemplating their immediate, short-term goals upon
graduation, higher levels of professional identification were posi-
tively associated with goals containing extrinsic content (i.e., fo-
Figure 2. Partially mediated model of mentoring to long-term career goals through professional identification.
p .05.
ⴱⴱ
p .01.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
51
MENTORING, IDENTIFICATION, AND CAREER GOALS
cused on obtaining rewards and positive evaluations from others),
with goals containing intrinsic content (i.e., seeking opportunities
to learn and grow and to do challenging, interesting, and impactful
work), and with setting goals of higher quality (i.e., personal goals
which are difficult, specific, and to which one is committed). We
also found that psychosocial mentoring was indirectly related to all
STG goal outcomes through professional identification. Taken
together, the sense of interpersonal belonging that develops from
psychosocial mentoring experiences enhances early professionals’
identification with the profession, which is then positively related
to goal content and setting high quality goals.
For the LTG model, professional identification positively re-
lated to goals with extrinsic and intrinsic content, but not to goal
quality. Instead, psychosocial mentoring had a positive direct
effect on LTG quality. Across both STG and LTG models, the
effect of the psychosocial mentoring on goals with extrinsic and
intrinsic content through professional identification was consis-
tent. However, differences across models arose in regard to the
relationship of psychosocial mentoring to goal quality. For short-
term goals, psychosocial mentoring was directly and indirectly,
through professional identification, related to goal quality. For
long-term goals, the effect of psychosocial mentoring on goal
quality was direct and not mediated by professional identification.
A potential theoretical explanation may be that the role-modeling
function of psychosocial mentoring provides early career profes-
sionals with a clear long-term vision of one’s future self, which is
more important to developing specific and challenging long-term
goals than is professional identification. However, we recommend
some caution in making comparisons between the STG and LTG
models because common method variance may explain the stron-
ger findings for the STG model as professional identification and
short-term goals were measured on the same survey (Podsakoff et
al., 2003).
Theoretical Implications
This study contributes to the limited work assessing the impor-
tance of identity development in role transition processes (Ash-
forth, 2001) by examining the role of mentoring in developing
professional identification in a sample of graduate students tran-
sitioning into the professional field. We found that psychosocial
mentoring, which we proposed fulfills the belonging motive, pos-
itively related to professional identification, whereas, career men-
toring, which we proposed fulfills the control motive was not
(unless psychosocial mentoring was excluded from the model). It
is possible that one reason for the lack of support between career
mentoring and professional identification is our study context.
Ashforth’s (2001) original model was conceptualized at the orga-
nizational rather than the professional level and professionals have
multiple targets to which they can relate. Control motives associ-
ated with mentors may relate to identity processes at the organi-
zational (university) level but not at the professional level. That is,
career mentoring may affect whether students feel they belong in
their particular program within the university, not whether they
belong in the profession itself.
We also contribute to social identity theory (Stryker & Burke,
2000) by including goal constructs as outcomes of identification
processes. Ashforth (2001) proposes that identity motives lead to
role identification which, in turn, leads to enacting a role identity
and to role-relevant outcomes, such as improved performance.
However, an important mediating step, especially for profession-
als, is the creation of group-relevant (e.g., organization, profes-
sion) goals as a precursor to performance. As such, an important
contribution of the current study is demonstrating that identity
process and identification are related to goals aligned with stan-
dards from one’s referent group. This finding is consistent with the
self-verification process in SIT in which goal-directed behavior is
driven to match the identity standard (Stryker & Burke, 2000).
This study contributes to goal setting theory by examining the
content and characteristics of goals in the career context. Setting
goals for one’s career is often done absent formal goal setting
mechanisms—yet this context has received sparse attention in both
the goal setting and careers literature (Greenhaus, 1987; Greenhaus
et al., 2010). Currently, the career context for goal setting largely
addresses the content of individual career goals through assessing
distance from career goals, goal importance, goal progress (e.g.,
Maier & Brunstein, 2001; Noe, 1996). We expanded on this
research by including goal quality, a concept integral to goal
setting theory, as an outcome. We find that both mentoring func-
tions and professional identification are related to the creation of
career goals which are specific, difficult, and to which one is
committed, the primary criteria for effective goals (Locke &
Latham, 1990). We also extend the goal-setting research by ex-
amining short-term and long-term goals separately. That psycho-
social mentoring differentially related to short-term and long-term
goal quality suggests future research should consider different
processes to explain how individuals develop quality short-term
versus long-term goals. This is especially important considering
that setting short-term (i.e., proximal) goals can affect success in
reaching long-term (i.e., distal) goals (Latham & Seijts, 1999).
Our last contribution is to organizational socialization theory
and the mentoring literature. As Allen et al. (2017) note, while
mentoring is often associated with socialization, “the process by
which this occurs is less understood” (p. 331). Combining two
socialization tactics (Jones, 1986; Van Maanen & Schein, 1979)
with Ashforth’s (2001) model of role transitions, we proposed that
career mentoring is representative of serial socialization tactics
through satisfying needs for control while psychosocial mentoring
is representative of investiture socialization tactics through satis-
fying needs for belonging. Investiture tactics (i.e., psychosocial
mentoring) were consistently and positively related to professional
identification and subsequent career goals. The direct and indirect
relationship from psychosocial mentoring to goal quality may help
explain why mentoring relates to career success (e.g., Allen et al.,
2004). Although previous work in the early career professional
context has shown that psychosocial mentoring is not related to the
number of future publications for graduate students, as a measure
of success (Green & Bauer, 1995; Paglis, Green, & Bauer, 2006),
it is possible that higher quality goals may target higher quality
publications, which may better capture career success.
Practical Implications
This study has practical implications for professional workers,
graduate students, and mentors. First, socialization into the pro-
fession is a process that begins within graduate school, and the
extent that an individual develops a relationship with the profes-
sion is in some ways determined by mentors (Feldman, 1976).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
52
GRECO AND KRAIMER
Early career professionals should recognize that their career goals,
or lack thereof, may be more shaped by psychosocial (i.e., inves-
titure) rather than career mentoring (i.e., serial) experiences. In
cases where mentoring and socialization practices are lacking,
such as when an assigned advisor is not providing adequate psy-
chosocial support, early career professionals may find it in their
best interest to proactively search for such support elsewhere.
Also, related to the supplementary analysis showing compensatory
effects for career mentoring, early career professionals who lack
psychosocial support can still expect career mentoring support to
relate to professional identification and related career goals.
Second, professional identification is positively related to both
short- and long-term extrinsic and intrinsic goals. When early
career professionals internalize the goals and objectives of the
profession, a way to solidify their standing in the profession is to
obtain a position in which they are highly paid but also able to
learn continuously and do meaningful work. This is consistent with
several studies of law and business students which have found that
students not only identified with their chosen occupation because
the profession represented the ability to have the “salary, prestige,
and lifestyle of the upper middle class” (Schleef, 2000, p. 156), but
also because it represented the ability to make a difference in the
world and do work that is inherently meaningful (Schleef, 2000).
Thus, professional workers should recognize the importance and
legitimacy in pursuit of both internal and external career goals.
Second, for graduate students and advisors it appears that pos-
itive psychosocial mentoring experiences such as role modeling,
acceptance, counseling, and friendship are more important for
creating a sense of professional identity than is career mentoring.
Accordingly, advisors should recognize the importance of provid-
ing friendship and other related behaviors for their protégés. Cau-
tion should be taken, however, when emphasizing the importance
of one type of mentoring over the other. Meta-analytic findings
reveal that psychosocial mentoring is more strongly related to
subjective measures of career success (i.e., career satisfaction, job
satisfaction) whereas career mentoring is more strongly related to
objective measures of career success (i.e., compensation, pro-
motion; Allen et al., 2004). Therefore, even though career
mentoring, in the presence of psychosocial mentoring, was not
related to professional identification or goals within the current
study, graduate students should still seek out mentor(s) who
fulfill both career and psychosocial mentoring roles as both
relate to career success.
Professionals and professional associations should recognize
that many graduate students pursue altruistic goals in addition to
furthering extrinsic career goals. Being motivated to pursue a
professional career in some part “for the money” does not imply
that all professional workers are materialistic and selfish; many
also have nonmaterialistic and generous goals. However, profes-
sional associations and universities could offer financial support
and emphasize the status of professionals who pursue lower sala-
ried jobs for more altruistic goals. For example, law associations
could provide grants for lawyers who become public defenders, or
professional associations could encourage donation of time/re-
sources for professional workers to provide services for individu-
als or organizations that may not usually be able to afford them. In
this way, professional associations may encourage goals that fur-
ther altruistic outcomes.
Limitations and Future Research
One strength of this study is the wide breadth of graduate
students sampled. Students included in the sample were pursuing
both Master’s degrees and PhDs and represented a wide variety of
fields in both the “hard” and “social” sciences. The wide coverage
of graduate students is a strength because the findings are more
directly generalizable to professionals from many fields including
business, engineering, computer science, and medicine. However,
one limitation of the study is the potential for differences between
students including whether students were enrolled part-time or
full-time. For example, part-time students may be less involved in
the profession compared to similar full-time students. Because we
did not measure for the enrollment status of respondents, we were
unable to control for this as a potential confounding effect. A
second limitation is the reliance on self-report data from these
graduate students. Same source bias can be problematic because
respondents, in an effort to appear consistent, may answer survey
questions in a way that leads to inflated relationships between
study variables (i.e., CMV; Podsakoff et al., 2003). We attempted
to minimize this effect by dividing the surveys across three time
periods. Further, based on the individual and affective nature of
mentoring experiences, identification, and career goals, we believe
that each individual respondent represents the most informed
source of information for the current study variables.
Another limitation in the study is the potential for reverse
causality between mentoring and professional identification. Indi-
viduals with higher levels of professional identification may seek
out help from mentors and thus, receive more mentoring. How-
ever, we believe that, in the context of professional socialization,
this relationship is less likely than the hypothesized one because
newcomers’ identification with the profession is theorized to de-
velop partly as a result of interacting with established members of
the profession, such as mentors (Ashforth, 2001). In addition,
mentoring is not directed only at newcomers with strong profes-
sional identification. More senior professionals may provide some
degree of career and psychosocial mentoring to newcomers who
do not identify with the profession out of a sense of professional
responsibility to guide and assist newcomers, collegial norms for
mentoring, or because the organization rewards mentoring of new-
comers.
Results from this study provide several avenues for future
research. The current study presented the first test of Ashforth’s
(2001) widely cited but rarely tested model of role transitions in
organizations. However, because we did not measure the motives
explicitly, one promising avenue for future research would be to
directly measure whether psychosocial mentoring helps fulfill be-
longing motives, and career mentoring fulfills control motives as
we proposed based on the model. Doing so would require first
developing and validating scales to measure the motives, but such
an effort would provide a stronger test of the underlying theory.
Future research can also address whether other variables besides
career and psychosocial mentoring fulfill these motives. For ex-
ample, we proposed a relationship between serial socialization and
the need for control and investiture socialization and the motive for
belonging. Other socialization tactics such as collective socializa-
tion through peer support may also relate to professional identifi-
cation. Last, future research could assess the same model in
regards to role transitions in organizations, rather than professions.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
53
MENTORING, IDENTIFICATION, AND CAREER GOALS
Ashforth’s (2001) model describes individuals entering jobs in
specific organizations, so it could be that the variables in the
present study are adequate reflections of identity motives, but they
may be more effective in identifying organizational, compared to
professional, identification processes.
Another future area of study could expand our understanding of
the motivation behind both extrinsic and intrinsic goals. For ex-
ample, it is possible that a professional sets an extrinsic goal to
have a high salary job for extrinsic reasons (e.g., “I want to live in
a fancy house”) or intrinsic reasons (e.g., “I want to be able to give
gifts to my favorite charities”). At the same time, it is possible for
a professional to set an intrinsic goal to make a positive impact on
society for intrinsic reasons (e.g., “I get a sense of fulfillment from
helping others”) or for extrinsic reasons (e.g., “Others evaluate me
better if I do charity work”). This is especially relevant considering
that graduate students equally endorsed extrinsic and intrinsic
goals. For professional and other types of individuals, future
research should examine the content and motivation behind both
extrinsic and intrinsic goals (e.g., Vansteenkiste, Lens, & Deci,
2006).
It would also be fruitful to examine how goals are related to
career-relevant outcomes. For example, a primary tenet of goal
setting theory is that higher quality goals lead to better perfor-
mance. It would be telling to see whether graduate students with
higher quality goals do indeed “succeed” more in their chosen
career. This, of course, raises the question of how to measure
whether or not one met one’s career goals. Career goals are likely
to change over time based on feedback from incremental goals
(Locke & Latham, 2002) or other career relevant events (Lee &
Mitchell, 1994; Seibert et al., 2013). Understanding the content
and quality of early career goals, assessing how they change over
time, and then whether any of those goals have been met produces
a challenging task for future research.
Conclusion
A defining characteristic of all professions is that they engage in
some type of advanced schooling. From the perspective that
schooling is the first step of socialization into a profession, grad-
uate school experiences can have distinct effects on professional
identification and career-related goals. This study examined how
mentoring, as an identity-related process, shaped the development
of career goals for early career professionals. The insights advance
theory related to identity and are useful for professional workers,
graduate students, and their advisors.
References
Abbott, A. (1981). Status and status strain in the professions. American
Journal of Sociology, 86, 819835. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/227318
Abbott, A. (2014). The system of professions: An essay on the division of
expert labor. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Abele, A. E., & Spurk, D. (2009). The longitudinal impact of self-efficacy
and career goals on objective and subjective career success. Journal of
Vocational Behavior, 74, 53–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2008.10
.005
Aguinis, H., & Vandenberg, R. J. (2014). An ounce of prevention is worth a
pound of cure: Improving research quality before data collection. Annual
Review of Organizational Psychology and Organizational Behavior, 1,
569–595. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-031413-091231
Allen, N. J., & Meyer, J. P. (1997). Commitment in the workplace: Theory,
research and application. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications.
Allen, T. D., Eby, L. T., Chao, G. T., & Bauer, T. N. (2017). Taking stock of
two relational aspects of organizational life: Tracing the history and shaping
the future of socialization and mentoring research. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 102, 324–337. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/apl0000086
Allen, T. D., Eby, L. T., Poteet, M. L., Lentz, E., & Lima, L. (2004). Career
benefits associated with mentoring for protégé: A meta-analysis. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 89, 127–136. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.89
.1.127
Altman, D. G. (1999). Practical statistics for medical research. New York,
NY: Chapman & Hall/CRC Press.
Amabile, T. M. (1993). Motivational synergy: Toward new conceptualizations
of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation in the workplace. Human Resource
Management Review, 3, 185–201. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/1053-4822(93)
90012-S
Arnold, H. J., & Feldman, D. C. (1982). A multivariate analysis of the
determinants of job turnover. Journal of Applied Psychology, 67, 350
360. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.67.3.350
Arthur, M. B., & Rousseau, D. M. (1996). The boundaryless career. New
York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Ashford, S. J., & Cummings, L. L. (1983). Feedback as an individual
resource: Personal strategies of creating information. Organizational
Behavior and Human Performance, 32, 370–398. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1016/0030-5073(83)90156-3
Ashforth, B. (2001). Role transitions in organizational life: An identity-
based perspective. Mahwah, NJ: Routledge.
Ashforth, B. E., & Mael, F. (1989). Social identity theory and the organi-
zation. Academy of Management Review, 14, 20–39. http://dx.doi.org/
10.5465/amr.1989.4278999
Ashforth, B. K., & Saks, A. M. (1996). Socialization tactics: Longitudinal
effects on newcomer adjustment. Academy of Management Journal, 39,
149–178.
Austin, A. E. (2002). Preparing the next generation of faculty: Graduate school
as socialization to the academic career. The Journal of Higher Education,
73, 94–122. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00221546.2002.11777132
Austin, A. E., & McDaniels, M. (2006). Preparing the professoriate of the
future: Graduate student socialization for faculty roles. In J. C. Smart
(Ed.), Higher education (pp. 397–456). Amsterdam, the Netherlands:
Springer.
Azizzadeh, A., McCollum, C. H., Miller, C. C., III, Holliday, K. M.,
Shilstone, H. C., & Lucci, A., Jr. (2003). Factors influencing career
choice among medical students interested in surgery. Current Surgery,
60, 210–213. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7944(02)00679-7
Baker, V. L., & Pifer, M. J. (2011). The role of relationships in the transition
from doctoral student to independent scholar. Studies in Continuing Edu-
cation, 33,
5–17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0158037X.2010.515569
Bandura,
A., & Schunk, D. H. (1981). Cultivating competence, self-
efficacy, and intrinsic interest through proximal self-motivation. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 586–598. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1037/0022-3514.41.3.586
Bandura, A., & Simon, K. M. (1977). The role of proximal intentions in
self-regulation of refractory behavior. Cognitive Therapy and Research,
1, 177–193. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01186792
Bauer, T. N., & Green, S. G. (1998). Testing the combined effects of
newcomer information seeking and manager behavior on socialization.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 72–83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
0021-9010.83.1.72
Becker, T. E. (2005). Potential problems in the statistical control of
variables in organizational research: A qualitative analysis with recom-
mendations. Organizational Research Methods, 8, 274–289. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1177/1094428105278021
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
54
GRECO AND KRAIMER
Bernerth, J. B., & Aguinis, H. (2016). A critical review and best-practice
recommendations for control variable usage. Personnel Psychology, 69,
229–283. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/peps.12103
Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Parent-child attachment and healthy
human development. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Brewer, M. B. (1993). Social identity, distinctiveness, and in-group homo-
geneity. Social Cognition, 11, 150–164. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/soco
.1993.11.1.150
Brunstein, J. C. (1993). Personal goals and subjective well-being: A
longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65,
1061–1070. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.65.5.1061
Brunstein, J. C., Schultheiss, O. C., & Grässmann, R. (1998). Personal
goals and emotional well-being: The moderating role of motive dispo-
sitions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 494–508.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.75.2.494
Cable, D. M., & Parsons, C. K. (2001). Socialization tactics and person-
organization fit. Personnel Psychology, 54, 1–23. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1111/j.1744-6570.2001.tb00083.x
Cantor, N., & Zirkel, S. (1990). Personality, cognition, and purposive
behavior. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and
research (pp. 135–164). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Chao, G. T., Walz, P. M., & Gardner, P. D. (1992). Formal and informal
mentorships: A comparison on mentoring functions and contrast with
nonmentored counterparts. Personnel Psychology, 45, 619636. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-6570.1992.tb00863.x
Credé, M., & Harms, P. D. (2015). 25 years of higher-order confirmatory
factor analysis in the organizational sciences: A critical review and
development of reporting recommendations. Journal of Organizational
Behavior, 36, 845–872. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/job.2008
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). The general causality orientations scale:
Self-determination in personality. Journal of Research in Personality,
19, 109–134. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0092-6566(85)90023-6
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits:
Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological
Inquiry, 11, 227–268. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01
DeShon, R. P., & Gillespie, J. Z. (2005). A motivated action theory account
of goal orientation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 1096–1127.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.90.6.1096
Dik, B. J., Sargent, A. M., & Steger, M. F. (2008). Career development
strivings assessing goals and motivation in career decision-making and
planning. Journal of Career Development, 35, 23–41. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1177/0894845308317934
Dreher, G. F., & Ash, R. A. (1990). A comparative study of mentoring
among men and women in managerial, professional, and technical
positions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75, 539–546. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/0021-9010.75.5.539
Dweck, C. S. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning. American
Psychologist, 41, 1040–1048. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.41
.10.1040
Eby, L. T., Butts, M., & Lockwood, A. (2003). Predictors of success in the
era of the boundaryless career. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 24,
689–708. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/job.214
Ellis, A. M., Nifadkar, S. S., Bauer, T. N., & Erdogan, B. (2017). Newcomer
adjustment: Examining the role of managers’ perception of newcomer
proactive behavior during organizational socialization. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 102, 993–1001. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/apl0000201
Farrell, A. M. (2010). Insufficient discriminant validity: A comment on
Bove, Pervan, Beatty, and Shiu (2009). Journal of Business Research,
63, 324–327. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2009.05.003
Featherman, D. L., & Hauser, R. M. (1976). Prestige or socioeconomic
scales in the study of occupational achievement? Sociological
Methods
& Research, 4, 402–422.
Feldman, D. C. (1976). A contingency theory of socialization. Administrative
Science Quarterly, 21, 433–452. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2391853
Fornell, C., & Larcker, D. F. (1981). Structural equation models with
unobservable variables and measurement error: Algebra and statistics.
Journal of Marketing Research, 18, 382–388. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
002224378101800313
Fugate, M., Kinicki, A. J., & Ashforth, B. E. (2004). Employability: A
psycho-social construct, its dimensions, and applications. Journal of
Vocational Behavior, 65, 14–38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2003.10
.005
Gould, S. (1979). Characteristics of career planners in upwardly mobile
occupations. Academy of Management Journal, 22, 539–550.
Granfield, R. (1992). Making elite lawyers: Visions of law at Harvard and
beyond. New York, NY: Routledge Press.
Green, S. G., & Bauer, T. N. (1995). Supervisory mentoring by advisers:
Relationships with doctoral student potential, productivity, and commit-
ment. Personnel Psychology, 48, 537–562. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j
.1744-6570.1995.tb01769.x
Greenhaus, J. H. (1987). Career management. Chicago, IL: Dryden Press.
Greenhaus, J. H., Callanan, G. A., & Godshalk, V. M. (2010). Career
management (4th ed.). Chicago, IL: Dryden Press.
Hekman, D. R., Bigley, G. A., Steensma, H. K., & Hereford, J. F. (2009).
Combined effects of organizational and professional identification on the
reciprocity dynamic for professional employees. Academy of Management
Journal, 52, 506–526. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/amj.2009.41330897
Hollenbeck, J. R., Klein, H. J., O’Leary, A. M., & Wright, P. M. (1989).
Investigation of the construct validity of a self-report measure of goal
commitment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 951–956. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/0021-9010.74.6.951
Howskins, E., & Ewens, A. (1999). How student’s experience professional
socialization. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 35, 41–49.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0020-7489(98)00055-8
Humberd, B. K., & Rouse, E. D. (2016). Seeing you in me and me in you:
Personal identification in the phases of mentoring relationships. Acad-
emy of Management Review, 41, 435–455. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/
amr.2013.0203
Johnson, R. E., Rosen, C. C., Chang, C., Djurdevic, E., & Taing, M. U.
(2012). Recommendations for improving the construct clarity of higher-
order multidimensional constructs. Human Resource Management Re-
view, 22, 62–72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.hrmr.2011.11.006
Johnson, R. E., Rosen, C. C., & Djurdjevic, E. (2011). Assessing the
impact of common method variance on higher order multidimensional
constructs. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96, 744–761. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/a0021504
Jones, G. R. (1986). Socialization tactics, self-efficacy, and newcomers’
adjustments to organizations. Academy of Management Journal, 29,
262–279.
Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1993). A dark side of the American dream:
Correlates of financial success as a central life aspiration. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 410422. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1037/0022-3514.65.2.410
Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1996). Further examining the American dream:
Differential correlates of intrinsic and extrinsic coals. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 280–287. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
0146167296223006
Kenward, M. G., & Molenberghs, G. (1998). Likelihood based frequentist
inference when data are missing at random. Statistical Science, 13,
236–247. http://dx.doi.org/10.1214/ss/1028905886
King, Z. (2004). Career self-management: Its nature, causes and conse-
quences. Journal
of Vocational Behavior, 65, 112–133. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/S0001-8791(03)00052-6
Kraimer, M. L., Seibert, S. E., Wayne, S. J., Liden, R. C., & Bravo, J.
(2011). Antecedents and outcomes of organizational support for devel-
opment: The critical role of career opportunities. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 96, 485–500. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0021452
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
55
MENTORING, IDENTIFICATION, AND CAREER GOALS
Kram, K. E. (1985). Improving the mentoring process. Training and
Development Journal, 39, 4043.
Kram, K. E. (1988). Mentoring at work: Developmental relationships in
organizational life. Lanham, MD: University Press.
Kram, K. E., & Isabella, L. A. (1985). Mentoring alternatives: The role of
peer relationships in career development. Academy of Management
Journal, 28, 110–132.
Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer
agreement for categorical data. Biometrics, 33, 159–174. http://dx.doi
.org/10.2307/2529310
Latham, G. P., & Seijts, G. H. (1999). The effects of proximal and distal goals
on performance on a moderately complex task. Journal of Organizational
Behavior, 20, 421–429. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-1379(1999
07)20:4421::AID-JOB8963.0.CO;2-#
Leary, M. R., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). The nature and function of
self-esteem: Sociometer theory. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in
experimental social psychology (Vol. 32, pp. 1–62). San Diego, CA:
Academic Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(00)80003-9
Lee, T. W., & Mitchell, T. R. (1994). An alternative approach: The unfolding
model of voluntary employee turnover. Academy of Management Review,
19, 51–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/amr.1994.9410122008
Lee, T. W., & Mowday, R. T. (1987). Voluntarily leaving an organization:
An empirical investigation of Steers and Mowday’s model of turnover.
Academy of Management Journal, 30, 721–743.
Levy, I., Kaplan, A., & Patrick, H. (2004). Early adolescents’ achievement
goals, social status, and attitudes towards cooperation with peers. Social
Psychology of Education, 7, 127–159. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/B:SPOE
.0000018547.08294.b6
Little, T. D., Cunningham, W. A., Shahar, G., & Widaman, K. F. (2002).
To parcel or not to parcel: Exploring the question, weighing the merits.
Structural Equation Modeling, 9, 151–173. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/
S15328007SEM0902_1
Locke, E. A. (1976). The nature and causes of job satisfaction. Handbook
of Industrial and Organizational Psychology, 1, 1297–1343.
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting & task
performance. Ann Arbor, MI: Prentice Hall, Inc.
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2002). Building a practically useful theory
of goal setting and task motivation. A 35-year odyssey. American
Psychologist, 57, 705–717. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.57.9
.705
London, M. (1983). Toward a theory of career motivation. Academy of
Management Review, 8, 620630. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/amr.1983
.4284664
Mael, F., & Ashforth, B. E. (1992). Alumni and their alma mater: A partial
test of the reformulated model of organizational identification. Journal
of Organizational Behavior, 13, 103–123. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/job
.4030130202
Maier, G. W., & Brunstein, J. C. (2001). The role of personal work goals
in newcomers’ job satisfaction and organizational commitment: A lon-
gitudinal analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 1034–1042.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.86.5.1034
Major, D. A., Kozlowski, S. W., Chao, G. T., & Gardner, P. D. (1995). A
longitudinal investigation of newcomer expectations, early socialization
outcomes, and the moderating effects of role development factors.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 80, 418431. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
0021-9010.80.3.418
Markus, H. (1983). Self-knowledge: An expanded view. Journal of Per-
sonality, 51, 543–565. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1983
.tb00344.x
Markus,
H., & Nurius, P. (1986). Possible selves. American Psychologist,
41, 954–969. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.41.9.954
McClelland, D. C. (1985). How motives, skills, and values determine what
people do. American Psychologist, 40, 812–825. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1037/0003-066X.40.7.812
Mirvis, P. H., & Hall, D. T. (1996). Psychological success and the bound-
aryless career. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 153, 65–380.
Mitchell, M. E., Eby, L. T., & Ragins, B. R. (2015). My mentor, my self:
Antecedents and outcomes of perceived similarity in mentoring relation-
ships. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 89, 1–9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
.jvb.2015.04.008
Morgan, M. (1985). Self-monitoring of attained subgoals in private study.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 623–630. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1037/0022-0663.77.6.623
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2007). Mplus user’s guide (6th ed.). Los
Angeles, CA: Author.
Nakao, K., & Treas, J. (1994). Updating occupational prestige and socio-
economic scores: How the new measures measure up. Sociological
Methodology, 24, 1–72. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/270978
Noe, R. A. (1996). Is career management related to employee development
and performance? Journal of Organizational Behavior, 17, 119–133.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-1379(199603)17:2119::AID-
JOB7363.0.CO;2-O
Noe, R. A., Noe, A. W., & Bachhuber, J. A. (1990). An investigation of the
correlates of career motivation. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 37,
340–356. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0001-8791(90)90049-8
Ostroff, C., & Kozlowski, S. W. (1992). Organizational socialization as a
learning process: The role of information acquisition. Personnel Psychol-
ogy, 45, 849874. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-6570.1992.tb00971.x
Paglis, L. L., Green, S. G., & Bauer, T. N. (2006). Does adviser mentoring
add value? A longitudinal study of mentoring and doctoral student
outcomes. Research in Higher Education, 47, 451–476. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1007/s11162-005-9003-2
Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J. Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003).
Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the
literature and recommended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology,
88, 879–903. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879
Pratt, M. G., & Rafaeli, A. (1997). Organizational dress as a symbol of
multilayered social identities. Academy of Management Journal, 40,
862–898. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/256951
Pratt, M. G., Rockmann, K. W., & Kaufmann, J. B. (2006). Constructing
professional identity: The role of work and identity learning cycles in the
customization of identity among medical residents. Academy of Manage-
ment Journal, 49, 235–262. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/amj.2006.20786060
Price, S. L. (2009). Becoming a nurse: A meta-study of early professional
socialization and career choice in nursing. Journal of Advanced Nursing,
65, 11–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2648.2008.04839.x
Roberson, L. (1989). Assessing personal work goals in the organizational
setting: Development and evaluation of the Work Concerns Inventory.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 44, 345–367.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0749-5978(89)90013-7
Roberson, L. (1990). Prediction of job satisfaction from characteristics of
personal work goals. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 11, 2941.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/job.4030110106
Schleef, D. (2000). “That’s a good question!” Exploring motivations for
law and business school choice. Sociology of Education, 73, 155–174.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2673214
Seibert, S. E., Kraimer, M. L., Holtom, B. C., & Pierotti, A. J. (2013). Even
the
best laid plans sometimes go askew: Career self-management pro-
cesses, career shocks, and the decision to pursue graduate education.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 98, 169–182. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
a0030882
Seijts, G. H., & Latham, G. P. (2001). The effect of distal learning,
outcome, and proximal goals on a moderately complex task. Journal of
Organizational Behavior, 22, 291–307. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/job.70
Seo, M. G., Barrett, L. F., & Bartunek, J. M. (2004). The role of affective
experience in work motivation. Academy of Management Review, 29,
423–439. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/amr.2004.13670972
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
56
GRECO AND KRAIMER
Steers, R. M. (1976). Factors affecting job attitudes in a goal setting
environment. Academy of Management Journal, 19, 6–16.
Stock, J., & Cervone, D. (1990). Proximal goal-setting and self-regulatory
processes. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14, 483–498. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1007/BF01172969
Stryker, S., & Burke, P. J. (2000). The past, present, and future of an
identity theory. Social Psychology Quarterly, 63, 284–297. http://dx.doi
.org/10.2307/2695840
Stryker, S., & Serpe, R. T. (1994). Identity salience and psychological
centrality: Equivalent, overlapping, or complementary concepts? Social
Psychology Quarterly, 57, 16–35. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2786972
Swann, W. B., Jr. (1983). Self-verifications: Bringing social reality into
harmony with the self. In J. Suls & A. G. Greenwald (Eds.), Social
psychological perspectives on the self (Vol. 2, pp. 33–66). Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Swann, W. B., Jr., & Read, S. J. (1981). Self-verification processes: How
we sustain our self-conceptions. Journal of Experimental Social Psy-
chology, 17, 351–372. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(81)90043-3
Swann, W. B., Jr., Stein-Seroussi, A., & Giesler, R. B. (1992). Why people
self-verify. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 392–401.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.62.3.392
Tajfel, H. E. (1978). Differentiation between social groups: Studies in the
social psychology of intergroup relations. London, UK: Academic Press.
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup
conflict. In W. G. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology of
intergroup relations (pp. 33–47). Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1985). The social identity theory of intergroup
behavior. In S. Worchel & W. G. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of inter-
group relations (2nd ed., pp. 7–24). Chicago, IL: Nelson-Hall.
Tenenbaum, H. R., Crosby, F. J., & Gliner, M. D. (2001). Mentoring
relationships in graduate school. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 59,
326–341. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jvbe.2001.1804
Treiman, D. J. (2013). Occupational prestige in comparative perspective.
New York, NY: Academic Press.
Turner, J. C. (1985). Social categorization and the self-concept: A social
cognitive theory of group behavior. In T. Postmes & N. R. Branscombe
(Eds.), Key readings in social psychology. Rediscovering social identity
(pp. 243–272). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Van Maanen, J. (1978). People processing: Strategies of organizational
socialization. Organizational Dynamics, 7, 19–36. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1016/0090-2616(78)90032-3
Van Maanen, J., & Schein, E. H. (1979). Toward a theory of organizational
socialization. In B. M. Staw (Ed.), Research in organizational behavior
(pp. 209–264). Greenwich, CT: JAI.
Vansteenkiste, M., Lens, W., & Deci, E. L. (2006). Intrinsic versus extrin-
sic goal contents in self-determination theory: Another look at the
quality of academic motivation. Educational Psychologist, 41, 19–31.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4101_4
Vignoles, V. L., Regalia, C., Manzi, C., Golledge, J., & Scabini, E. (2006).
Beyond self-esteem: Influence of multiple motives on identity construc-
tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 308–333. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.90.2.308
Wayne, S. J., Liden, R. C., Kraimer, M. L., & Graf, I. K. (1999). The role of
human capital, motivation and supervisor sponsorship in predicting career
success. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 20, 577–595. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-1379(199909)20:5577::AID-JOB9583.0.CO;
2-0
Williams, G. C., Hedberg, V. A., Cox, E. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Extrinsic
life goals and health-risk behaviors in adolescents. Journal of Applied Social
Psychology, 30, 1756–1771. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2000
.tb02466.x
Williams, L. J., & O’Boyle, E. H., Jr. (2008). Measurement models for
linking latent variables and indicators: A review of human resource
management research using parcels. Human Resource Management Re-
view, 18, 233–242. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.hrmr.2008.07.002
Received March 27, 2018
Revision received April 24, 2019
Accepted April 24, 2019
E-Mail Notification of Your Latest Issue Online!
Would you like to know when the next issue of your favorite APA journal will be available
online? This service is now available to you. Sign up at https://my.apa.org/portal/alerts/ and you will
be notified by e-mail when issues of interest to you become available!
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
57
MENTORING, IDENTIFICATION, AND CAREER GOALS