Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=plcp21
Download by: [Macquarie University Library] Date: 16 January 2017, At: 13:59
Language, Cognition and Neuroscience
ISSN: 2327-3798 (Print) 2327-3801 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/plcp21
What can functional Transcranial Doppler
Ultrasonography tell us about spoken language
understanding?
N. A. Badcock & M. A. Groen
To cite this article: N. A. Badcock & M. A. Groen (2017): What can functional Transcranial
Doppler Ultrasonography tell us about spoken language understanding?, Language, Cognition
and Neuroscience, DOI: 10.1080/23273798.2016.1276608
To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2016.1276608
Published online: 16 Jan 2017.
Submit your article to this journal
View related articles
View Crossmark data
What can functional Transcranial Doppler Ultrasonography tell us about spoken
language understanding?
N. A. Badcock
a,b
and M. A. Groen
c
a
ARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, Department of Cognitive Science, Macquarie University, North Ryde, Australia;
b
Perception and Action Research Centre, Macquarie University, North Ryde, Australia;
c
Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University,
Nijmegen, The Netherlands
ABSTRACT
This review describes language research conducted using the neurophysiological imaging
technique, functional Transcranial Doppler Ultrasound (fTCD). FTCD estimates the blood flow
velocity in the cerebral arteries from which, neural activity is inferred. The review provides a brief
history and introduction to fTCD, including data acquisition, task design, and data processing.
Challenges and solutions for the use of fTCD for language research are covered, reporting on
production and comprehension paradigms, task difficulty and behavioural performance during
covert and overt speech production, and participant characteristics (age and sex). We note the
limited application of fTCD to the topic of spoken language understanding, commenting on the
value of examining lateralisation in this endeavour, as well as the advantages of its use, namely
portability and low cost, to supplement other imaging techniques.
ARTICLE HISTORY
Received 15 February 2016
Accepted 16 December 2016
KEYWORDS
Speech; language;
lateralisation; Doppler;
functional Transcranial
Doppler ultrasound
1. Historical background
Following the first reports of extracranial blood flow vel-
ocity (BFV) record ings using Doppler ultrasound (Miya-
zaki & Kato, 1965; Satomura & Kaneko, 1960), Aaslid,
Markwalder, and Nornes et al. (1982) pioneered its use
for intracranial blood vessels. They overcame the
attenuative qualities of bone and soft tissues by using
lower frequency ultrasound (12 MHz) and focusing on
(or insonating) vessels through the temporal bone
windows the thinnest skull region (see Figure 1(A)). In
this way, BFVs were measured non-invasively in the
middle, anterior, and posterior cerebral arteries (Aaslid
et al., 1982), leading to widespread medical applications.
More recently, researchers began to time-lock this
activity to cognitive tasks known as functional Tran-
scranial Doppler Ultrasonography (fTCD) taking unilat-
eral (Droste, Harders, & Rastogi, 1989) and bilateral
measurements (Hartje, Ringelstein, Kistinger, Fabianek,
& Willmes, 1994; Rihs et al., 1995; Silvestrini, Cupini,
Matteis, Troisi, & Caltagirone, 1994) with a goal of asses-
sing cerebral lateralisation of cognitive abilities rela-
tively greater task-related activation of one
hemisphere, compared to the other hemisphere.
However, these early fTCD results with verbal and non-
verbal tasks were ambiguous and lacked sufficient
reliability to draw conclusions on an individual basis.
Subsequent developments in experimental design (e.g.
Knecht et al., 1996) and analysis (Deppe, Knecht, Hen-
ningsen, & Ringelstein, 1997), notably improved the sen-
sitivity of fTCD and facilitated its use as a clinical and
research tool for studying lateralisation.
Clinically, fTCD is used in pre-surgical evaluations of
epilepsy patients (e.g. Knake et al., 2003), and in follow-
up measurements with a range of patient populations
(Knake et al., 2006), but studying language lateralisation
is also of theoretical importance. A lateralised brain is
thought to process information more efficiently (Rogers
& Vallortigara, 2015; Vallortigara & Rogers, 2005), but
associations are unclear in humans: atypical language
lateralisation has not been associated with behavioural
impairments in adults (Knecht et al., 2001). However,
fTCD (Bishop, Holt, Whitehouse, & Groen, 2014;Illingworth
&Bishop,2009; Whitehouse & Bishop, 2009) and fMRI
(Badcock, Bishop, Hardiman, Barry, & Watkins, 2012;de
Guibert et al., 2011; Sun, Lee, & Kirby, 2010)researchhas
repeatedly reported weaker language lateralisation to be
more common in individuals with developmental
language and literacy impairments. Bishop (2013;Bishop
et al., 2014) discusses several possible explanations for
this conundrum, but currently, the evidence is indecisive.
As
sessing language lateralisation directly (rather than
relying on a behavioural proxy), in large samples of indi-
viduals varying in language proficiency and across devel-
opment is needed to shed light on this issue, and fTCD is a
fitting technique in this endeavour.
© 2017 Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group
CONTACT N. A. Badcock [email protected]
LANGUAGE, COGNITION AND NEUROSCIENCE, 2017
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2016.1276608
2. Overview of the method
2.1. The neurophysiology of fTCD
Via insonating a blood vessel through the temporal bone
windows of the skull (i.e. transcranially; see Figure 1), BFV
(cm/s) can be determined be comparing frequency
changes of the transmitted and returned ultrasound
signals reflected against the moving blood cells (i.e. the
well-known Doppler effect). As cerebral BFV increases
in response to neural firing in order to maintain
resources to the cells (Escartin & Rouach, 2013;Li&
Freeman, 2015; Villringer & Dirnagl, 1995), neural activity
in the brain regions supplied by the insonated vessel can
be inferred. The middle cerebral artery (MCA) is the most
commonly insonated vessel in language research, sup-
plying blood to approximately 50% of the cortex (van
der Zwan, Hillen, Tulleken, & Dujovny, 1993), including
areas linked to language processing (see Table 1). There-
fore, when someone produces speech, there will be an
accompanying increase in BFV in the MCA which can
be measured using fTCD. See Bishop, Badcock, and
Holt (2010) for a video demonstration of the procedure
and Figure 2 for its relation to other neurophysiological
methods.
2.2. Data acquisition
An experienced user can insonate vessels in less than 5
minutes but the location is variable and, as a result,
setup time can be longer (for a setup guide, including
depth of insonation, see Badcock, Spooner, et al.,
2016). One caveat to the fTCD procedure is that
insonation of a vessel may fail in 510% of people (in
the authors experiences and given in Lohmann, Ringel-
stein, & Knecht, 2006), 9% of children (016 years, Iova
et al., 2004), and up to 25% in older adults (i.e. 55-years
and over: M = 71; Bakker et al., 2004 ). Failure is due to
the thickness and density of the temporal bone
window, which tends to affect women (i.e. thicker) and
older adults (i.e. less density leads to greater signal
refraction) with higher likelihood (Wijnhoud, Franckena,
van der Lugt, Koudstaal, & Dippel, 2008); medication
also affects bone thickness (Kattan, 1970; Lefebvre,
Haining, & Labbé, 1972). Once setup, session duration
is task dependent, typically requiring between 30 and
60 seconds per trial. Although not systematically investi-
gated (but see Badcock, Spooner, et al., 2016,for
reliability at varying numbers of trials), typical studies
include 20 or more trials, lasting between 10 and
20 minutes.
2.3. Task design
With regard to language research, fTCD has mainly been
used to study lateralisation. Bilateral monitoring makes it
particularly suited to this purpose. To illustrate the key
variables, we focus on the gold standard language later-
alisation task in fTCD research: word generation. For this
task, participants are asked to silently generate words
beginning with a visually presented letter (see
Figure 3). This is preceded by a 5-seconds preparatory
period with a Clear Mind instruction. Words are gener-
ated for 15 seconds, followed by a 5-second period of
overt report to ascertain task compliance. A 35-second
Figure 1. Panel A: A cartoon diagram of the left temporal window, underlying middle cerebral artery (MCA), and region supplied by
MCA. Panel B: Example insonation of the left and right middle cerebral arteries, including the headmount with the probes fitted. Please
note this is for illustration purposes only. For photographs and diagrams of the variability of temporal windows see Ringelstein,
Kahlscheuer, Niggemeyer, and Otis (1990 ). There is also a free interactive simulator that provides clear diagrams (available for
Windows operating systems from Haemodynamics AG, http://www.transcranial.com/edu/download.html).
2 N. A. BADCOCK AND M. A. GROEN
period of relaxation follows to return BFV to a resting
state (i.e. normalisation): a baseline against which acti-
vation can be compared. Each trial lasts for 60 seconds.
The task, pioneered by Knecht et al. (1996 ), is reliable
(Knecht, Deppe, Ringelstein, et al., 1998), and has been
validated against the Wada technique (Knecht, Deppe,
Ebner, et al., 1998) and fMRI (Deppe et al., 2000;
Somers et al., 2011). These comparisons, as well as its
extensive use in the literature, set word generation as a
standard paradigm for fTCD research.
The required elements of fTCD paradigms include nor-
malisation/baseline, preparation, and activation. Normal-
isation should be included before and after an event to
ensure that activity is sufficiently separated from adjacent
events. However, normalisation duration varies between
studies. Gutierrez-Sigut, Payne, and MacSweeney (2015)
used a 10-second normalisation, whereas Badcock, Nye,
and Bishop (2012) used 25 seconds. Despite Gutierrez-
Sigut et al. and Badcock et al. reporting typical distri-
butions of lateralisation, the task reliability was low
(split-half r = 0.61 and Cronbachs α = 0.52, respectively)
compared with canonical replications of word generation
(e.g. r = 0.89; Bishop, Watt, & Papadatou-Pastou, 2009).
Although likely that some minimum is required, this has
not been investigated. Alternative strategies to the
relax normalisation have been implemented, for
example, watching a to-be-described video (i.e. anima-
tion description; Bishop et al., 2009) which is more
engaging for children and reported to result in non-later-
alised activity. This may be useful for lateralisation
research but for other applications, especially with
adults, less engaging normalisation may be best.
Task preparation is cued by a brief tone usually
accompanied by Clear Mind text presented for 5
seconds. The presence of the tone has been demon-
strated to increase the magnitude of the change in BFV
in the predicted direction; that is, greater left velocity
for word generation (Knecht et al., 1996); though again,
the text instruction has not been investigated.
Activation is cued by the presentation of a letter to
which participants are instructed to silently generate
words. Silent generation of words was originally encour-
aged to avoid movement artefacts, however, overt tasks
have been successfully employed without issue (e.g.
Gutierrez-Sigut, Payne, et al., 2015). The early work
encouraged the production of four words per letter
(Knecht et al., 1996), later adjusted to as many as you
can (Knecht, Deppe, Ebner et al., 1998). Presumably
this extension results in greater activation consistency
between trials and therefore internal reliability, although
this has not been tested.
The duration of normalisation and activation periods
allows time for change in BFV to plateau (i.e. approxi-
mately 10 seconds; Rosengarten, Osthaus, & Kaps,
2002); however, the required time is derived from a para-
digm without a preparation period. As noted by Knecht
Table 1. Description of the cortical coverage and relevance for language (from Price, 2010) of brain regions supplied by the left middle
cerebral artery. The coverage is based upon examination of 50 hemispheres from 25 post-mortem brains, and the description is
minimally adapted from Gibo, Carver, Rhoton, Lenkey, and Mitchell ( 1981).
Lobe Coverage Gyrus
Brodmann
area Relevance for language
Frontal Lateral half of the orbital surface and the area between the
Sylvian fissure below, the superior frontal sulcus with
frequent overlap onto the superior frontal gyrus above,
and the central sulcus posteriorly and near, but stopping
short of the frontal tip anteriorly. The branches of the MCA
did not reach the superior margin nor the medial surface
Pars orbitalis 47 Semantic retrieval processes
Pars triangularis 45 Word selection
(comprehension and
production)
Pars opercularis 44 Hierarchical sequencing and
articulatory planning
Middle frontal 46 Word retrieval (more in
production)
Precentral 6, 4 Initiation and execution of
movement Sensorimotor
interface
Parietal Bounded anteriorly by the central sulcus, inferiorly by the
Sylvian fissure, and superiorly by the inferior half of the
superior parietal lobule. Posteriorly, the area extended
backward onto the lateral surface of the occipital lobe
Postcentral 3, 1, 2 Phonological retrieval/covert
articulation Semantic
constraints
Inferior and superior parietal
lobules (including the
supramarginal and angular gyri)
40, 39
Temporal Entire lateral surface except for a small posteroinferior strip.
In addition, it supplied the lateral part of the inferior
surface of the temporal lobe, the temporal pole, the uncus,
and adjacent part of the parahippocampal gyrus. Branches
frequently extended onto the lateral surface of the
occipital lobe
Superior temporal (including
Heschls gyrus and the Planum
Temporale)
41, 42, 22 Auditory input Prelexical
auditory objects
Sensorimotor integration
Middle temporal 21 Semantic processing of single
words
Inferior temporal 20 Amodal semantic combinations
Temporal pole 38 Intelligible speech/amodal
semantic combinations
Occipital Branches supplying the parietal and temporal lobes
overlapped onto the lateral occipital gyri, but they did not
extend to the occipital pole
Lateral occipital gyri 19
LANGUAGE, COGNITION AN D NEUROSCIENCE 3
et al. (1996), peak change following cuing occurs at
around 4 seconds. Shorter activation periods have
been used; for example, single word report in less
than 5 seconds to brief (3 to 5 word) definitions
(Badcock, Nye, et al., 2012). Although the lateralisation
results were comparable to word generation, the
relationship between the two tasks was weak. Further
investigation to determine optimal task parameters is
warranted.
It is worth noting that fTCD language research uses
blocked designs, in contrast to rapid event-related
designs used with fMRI (DEsposito, Zarahn, & Aguirre,
1999) or EEG (i.e. event-related potentials; Luck, 2014).
Theoretically, rapid event-related fTCD designs are poss-
ible, however, to our knowledge, this has not been
tested.
2.4. Data processing and analysis
FTCD data are processed in a number of steps to cal cu-
late event-related change in BFV (see Table 2 and
Deppe, Knecht, Lohmann, & Ringelstein, 2004; Deppe
et al., 1997, for further details). Example group data for
word generation are displayed in Figure 3. Critically,
the difference between the left and right velocities
shows an increase from 5 to 15 seconds, indicating left
lateralisation at the group level. Whilst the primary
purpose of the documented processing is for laterality
index calculation (see Table 2), the timing and amplitude
of left-right average, left and right independent (e.g. in a
vigilance experiment, Schultz, Matthews, Warm, & Wash-
burn, 2009), or single channel changes in velocity may
address new questions.
Figure 2. Schematic of the spatial resolution and temporal relationship between active (squares) and passive (ovals) neurophysiological
methods and physiological activity (Deppe, Ringelstein, & Knecht, 2004; Walsh & Cowey, 2000). Methods: transcranial direct current
stimulation (tDCS), transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), magnoencephalography (MEG), electroencephalography (EEG), positron
emission tomomography (PET), functional near infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS), fun ctional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), functional
Transcranial Doppler Ultrasound (fTCD).
Figure 3. Blood flow velocity (% change in cm/s) to silent word generation (a latency of 0 corresponds to letter presentation to cue
generation). Mean activity for a group (n = 17) are presented for the left and right middle cerebral arteries, and the left minus right
difference. Baseline and period of interest timings are marked, along with the peak difference (vertical bar within the period of interest).
A schematic of the task elements is displayed below the x-axis, including a period of relaxation to establish baseline blood flow velocity,
a preparatory cue to Clear Mind, a letter stimulus to cue silent word generation beginning with the presented letter, a period of silent
word generation, followed by overt report of the words (i.e. Say), and then relaxation to induce normalisation of blood flow velocity.
The laterality index is 2.07% change from baseline [95% confidence intervals: 1.99, 2.14].
4 N. A. BADCOCK AND M. A. GROEN
Deppe and colleagues developed the processing
steps, easily implemented with their software Average
(described in Deppe et al., 1997, 2004). Average is
Windows-based software, and a cross-platform
implementation of the methods is available with the
MATLAB toolbox, dopOSCCI (introduced in Badcock,
Holt, Holden, & Bishop, 2012). The toolbox allows for cus-
tomisation and extension to the steps, and introduced
activation correction for extreme values as well as rejec-
tion of extreme values based on the left minus right
difference (see Badcock, Spooner, et al., 2016).
At the individual level, changes in BFV have been ana-
lysed using comparison of indices to zero using 95% con-
fidence intervals (e.g. Groen, Whitehouse, Badcock, &
Bishop, 2013) and analyses of variance (e.g. Badcock,
Nye, et al., 2012).
3. Challenges and solutions for studying
spoken language
With the focus of fTCD language research on lateralisa-
tion and its potential use as a clinical tool in epilepsy
surgery, common language tasks for fTCD require the
production of words (e.g. word generation, naming) or
sentences (e.g. sentence construction, picture, or anima-
tion description). Although tasks vary in terms of the
amount of auditory input and comprehension
demands, few studies have investigated understanding
of spoken language per se. In the following sections,
we discuss existing use of receptive language tasks in
fTCD research, and how fTCD responses to language
tasks are associated with task difficulty and performance,
and participant characteristics.
3.1. Types of language tasks
Research has compared comprehension and language
production using listening to short stories (jokes,
poems, or everyday life events) versus producing short
stories from a picture cue (Stroobant, Van Boxstael, & Vin-
gerhoets, 2011), and sentence judgements versus word
generation (Buchinger et al., 2000). Consistent with
work using the Wada technique (e.g. Boatman et al.,
1998) and fMRI (e.g. Tzourio-Mazoyer, Josse, Crivello, &
Mazoyer, 2004), receptive tasks are less strongly latera-
lised than expressive tasks (Buchinger et al., 2000; Stroo-
bant et al., 2011). However, the expressive and receptive
tasks in this research were poorly matched for auditory
input or linguistic content, therefore lateralisation differ-
ences could be due to increased bilateral involvement in
multiple processes (e.g. phonological, syntactic, or
semantic knowledge). In a comparison of listening to
stories versus noise or melody, Carod Artal, Vazquez
Cabrera, and Horan (2004) reported a larger increase in
Table 2. Summary of processing and analysis steps for fTCD data (for further details see Deppe et al., 1997).
Step Description Comment
Downsampling Data are usually recorded at 100 Hz and downsampled to
25 Hz (1 sample every 40 ms)
Historically this step was required as computing power was limited,
in addition to the fact that the blood flow response is slow so
millisecond accuracy is overkill. Given the power of modern
computers, this step may be skipped, however, downsampling will
increase the speed of processing
Normalisation Data for each channel are transformed to have a mean of 100 The angle of the ultrasound probe will likely differ between left and
right channels, resulting in differences in overall velocity.
Normalisation corrects for this difference. This removes any intra-
individual differences as well resting state differences between
channels
Heart Cycle
Integration
Fluctuations in velocity due to the heart cycle (i.e. pulse) are
removed by averaging across individual cycles
This is a form a data cleaning that does not introduce artefacts
associated with bandpass filtering and improves the frequency
distribution of the data (from bimodal to unimodal), rendering it
suitable for traditional statistical interrogation. See Pinaya et al.
(2015) where this is not included and Badcock, Pascoe, and Groen
(2016) for a response to this
Epoching The continuous recording is divided into event-related time
periods
Data Screening Epochs with extreme values are excluded from further analysis Extreme data is considered a recording artefact, often due to probe
displacement caused by movement. This can be identified as a)
values beyond a certain range (e.g. ±50 cm/s beyond the mean of
the data) or b) a channel difference outside the expected range
(e.g. 20 cm/s where the average is usually less than 5 cm/s)
Baseline Correction Subtraction of average activity during a control period from all data
within an epoch, for the left and right channels separately
Requires the specification of a baseline time period within the epoch,
during which rest or control-task activity is assumed. The
percentage change in activation due to the task can be inferred.
Periods of 4 to 10 seconds have been used in the literature (10 in
Bishop et al., 2009; in Gutierrez-Sigut, Daws, et al., 2015) the
effects have not been compared systematically
Laterality Index
Calculation
The peak left minus right channel activation difference within a
period of interest is determined. The average activation within a
2-second time-window around this peak is the laterality index
The period of interest is selected in relation to task-onset typically
with a 5-second delay to allow velocity to peak. Left activation is
reflected by positive values, right is negative
LANGUAGE, COGNITION AN D NEUROSCIENCE 5
left lateralisation to stories. In this case, stimulus com-
plexity varied between conditions, complicating
interpretation of the results. Although most fTCD work
has investigated lateralisation for language as if
language were a unidimensional construct, Gutierrez-
Sigut, Daws, et al. (2015 , 2015) compared the traditional
word generation task (i.e. phonological fluency) with a
semantic fluency equivalent and did not find differences
in direction or degree in lateralisation indices. In contrast,
Stroobant, Buijs, and Vingerhoets (2009) compared tasks
tapping multiple linguistic processes and found stronger
left lateralisation for tasks involving word generation
(phonological fluency) or syntactic processes (sentence
construction) than one involving semantic (synonimity)
judgements.
To date, fTCD has been used for relatively crude categ-
orisation in tasks with high ecological validity, but poor
on experimental control, and which mostly conceptual-
ise language as a unidimensional construct. To increase
our understanding of lateralisation of language and
the utility of fTCD to study it it is important to
address the following issues. Firstly, experimental
control over a range of factors (e.g. task difficulty) that
influence lateralisation is poor. Secondly, lateralisation
for language is predominantly treated as a unidimen-
sional construct, however, as illustrated by Stroobant
et al. (2009), degree of la teralisation can vary between
language tasks. It remains an outstanding question
whether a single laterality measure is suitable to sum-
marise activity across tasks, or whether individual vari-
ation between language tasks is meaningful. Carefully
matching task properties tapping different linguistic
domains and processes in both production and compre-
hension, and recognising individual differences between
these domains are important next steps.
3.2. Associations with task difficulty and
performance
Task difficulty is one parameter that might influence
fTCD-estimated lateralisation, but results have been
inconsistent. Dräger and Knecht (2002) manipulated
the difficultly of word generation by providing partici-
pants with letters forming the beginnings of words, con-
trasting the frequency of available items (i.e. high = easy,
versus low = hard). Although behavioural accuracy
matched retrieval difficulty, fTCD outcomes did not: con-
sistent with other fTCD work with language (Badcock,
Nye, et al., 2012) and spatial ability (Rosch, Bishop, &
Badcock, 2012). In an fMRI follow-up, Dräger et al.
(2004) suggested that lack of suitable cerebral territory
supplied by the MCA rendered fTCD insensitive to their
difficulty manipulation: parietal regions outside this
territory were highlighted. However, recently, pace of
decision-making was evident using fTCD. Payne, Gutier-
rez-Sigut, Subik, Woll, and MacSweeney (2015) manipu-
lated the number of word-pair rhyme and line
orientation judgments required in a 17.5-second
period: 5 or 10. More judgments were associated with
stronger lateralisation for rhyme (greater left) and line
(greater right) tasks. Therefore, fTCD is sensitive to
some aspects of task difficulty.
The behaviour-lateralisation relationship is important
for interpreting silent word generation tasks comparing
groups that may differ in language abilities (e.g. dyslexia,
Illingworth & Bishop, 2009). In such studies, it is imposs-
ible to establish whether the behavioural differences
underpin neural differences this concern is supported
by a lack of relationship between the number of words
reported and fTCD measureme nts in word generation
(Badcock, Nye, et al., 2012). However, Gutierrez-Sigut,
Payne, et al. (2015) have demonstrated similar activation
for covert and overt speech, observing a significant cor-
relation between behaviour and fTCD lateralisation for
overt speech. Therefore, overt speech paradigms are
recommended.
3.3. Associations with participant characteristics
Lateralisation is also influenced by participant character-
istics, such as age, sex or, relatedly, menstrual cycle.
Regarding age, one fTCD study in children (15 years)
reported greater left lateralisation at younger ages
(Kohler et al., 2015); however, the majority (ages
ranging from 2 to 16) report no association (Groen,
Whitehouse, Badcock, & Bishop, 2012; Haag et al., 2010;
Hodgson, Hirst, & Hudson, 2016; Lohmann, Dräger,
Müller-Ehrenberg, Deppe, & Knecht, 2005; Stroobant
et al., 2011) which is at odds with fMRI (Gaillard et al.,
2000; Holland et al., 2001, 2007; Szaflarski, Schmithorst,
et al., 2006; Szaflarski, Holland, Schmithorst, & Byars,
2006). This discrepancy could be explained by greater
sensitivity in fMRI to area-specific age-related changes
or by the fTCD tasks used. As Holland et al. (2007)
suggested, assessing late-acquired language skills may
result in age-related differences in lateralisation, but
the fTCD tasks typically require description of simple pic-
tures or animation, probing early-acquired skills. At the
other end of the spectrum, older participants showed
reduced left-lateralised activation during a word gener-
ation task (6075 year-olds, Keage et al., 2015).
Concerning sex,
1
despite a long-standing debate on
malefemale language lateralisation differences, empiri-
cal support for more bilateral language in women is
lacking (Sommer, Aleman, Bouma, & Kahn, 2004;
Sommer, Aleman, Somers, Boks, & Kahn, 2008), or
6 N. A. BADCOCK AND M. A. GROEN
effects are very small, and possi bly age-dependent (Hirn-
stein, Westerhausen, Korsnes, & Hugdahl, 2013). This is in
line with a lack of sex differences reported in fTCD
studies (e.g. Knecht et al., 2000; Whitehouse & Bishop,
2009). Interestingly, a recent study evaluating the test
retest reliability of lateralisation using fTCD across
several weeks, found laterality indices were much more
variable in women (Helmstaedter, Jockwitz, & Witt,
2015); specifically, a relative shift towards bilateral acti-
vation in women at menstrual cycle onset. This finding
demands consideration of menstrual cycle when asses-
sing lateralisation and it may explain contrasting findings
on sex differences, and confound existing between
group research. Therefore, both age and sex are impor-
tant factors in lateralisation research and may be impor-
tant for fTCD research per se.
4. Advantages and future directions
Language research with fTCD is in its infancy, predomi-
nantly applied in clinical settings, using ecologically valid,
but poorly controlled paradigms, probing multiple
aspects of language simultaneously. As such, there are
no key empirical contributions to the understanding of
spoken language yet. Nevertheless, fTCD is worth consider-
ation as the field is ripe for paradigm development and
refinements in analysis, including considering measures
beyond laterality indices. These developments enable
the advancements of our understanding of language later-
alisation for production and comprehension.
4.1. Advantages of fTCD
Although its spatial resolution is limited (see Figure 2 and
Table 1), fTCD has several advantages, compared to other
techniques. It is highly portable and relatively inexpen-
sive (Pelletier, Sauerwein, Lepore, Saint-Amour, & Las-
sonde, 2007), making it a useful screening tool for
investigations requiring large sample-sizes, such as
genetic studies (e.g. Somers et al., 2015). Additionally,
its robustness to articulation and gross movements,
and participant friendly administration, make it well
suited for use with young children, older adults, and
patient groups. Indeed, adaptations of the gold standard
word generation t ask eliciting overt sentence production
in response to pictures (Haag et al., 2010; Lohmann et al.,
2005) or animations (Bishop et al., 2009) or picture
naming (Badcock et al., 2016; Kohler et al., 2015) have
resulted in reliable measurements of language lateralisa-
tion. Moreover, the nature of the ultrasound signal
makes it appropriate for research where other tech-
niques are not, such as in individuals with cochlear
implants (e.g. Chilosi et al., 2014). As fTCD is non-invasive
and can be administered repeatedly in the same partici-
pants, there are opportunities to examine and project
recovery from stroke (for e.g. in motor control see
Sarkar, Ghosh, Ghosh, & Collier, 2007). The advantages
of fTCD low-cost, portability, robustness to articulation
and gross movements, and participant friendly adminis-
tration support its use as an imaging technique for
language research in the foreseeable future, supple-
menting weaknesses of other techniques.
4.2. Future developments in task design and data
analysis
As mentioned, there are a number of task parameters yet
to be optimised for fTCD. This concerns all phases of a trial:
normalisation, preparation, and activation (see Section
2.3). There are outstanding questions regarding the influ-
ence of task instruction on preparation and behaviour.
Also, the number of required trials and the possibility of
adopting a rapid event-related (instead of a blocked)
design, merit investigation. Refining matching of stimulus
properties across conditions to equate demands between
production and comprehension tasks is needed to inves-
tigate whether a unidimensional view of language latera-
lisation is justified. Regarding analysis, we are yet to
optimise data cleaning techniques to maximise the
signal to noise ratio (Badcock, Spooner, et al., 2016)and
variables beyond the peak difference should be con-
sidered (e.g. trajectory of BFV increase to infer neural sub-
trates; Meyer, Spray, Fairlie, & Uomini, 2014). Following a
different approach, resting TCD can be used to investigate
cerebrovascular functioning (Keage et al., 2012). This
approach has associated poorer cerebrovascular function-
ing to decreased fluid, but not crystallised, intelligence in
aging populations (Keage et al., 2015). These develop-
ments offer exciting potential for the use of fTCD for the
investigation of spoken language.
Note
1. Here, we refer to the dichotomous variable sex. We note
that the continuous variable of hormones levels will
likely be the more accurate advancement for this
research (e.g. Hausmann, Slabbekoorn, Van Goozen,
Cohen-Kettenis, & Güntürkün, 2000).
Acknowledgements
Thanks to Heather Payne, Paul Sowman, and Alexandra
Woolgar for feedback on the work.
Disclosure statement
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.
LANGUAGE, COGNITION AN D NEUROSCIENCE 7
ORCID
N. A. Badcock http://orcid.org/0000-0001-6862-4694
M. A. Groen
http://orcid.org/0000-0002-6178-2937
References
Aaslid, R., Markwalder, T.-M., & Nornes, H. (1982). Noninvasive
transcranial Doppler ultrasound recording of flow velocity
in basal cerebral arteries. Journal of Neurosurgery, 57(6),
769774. doi:10.3171/jns.1982.57.6.0769
Badcock, N. A., Bishop, D. V. M., Hardiman, M. J., Barry, J. G., &
Watkins, K. E. (2012). Co-localisation of abnormal brain
structure and function in specific language impairment.
Brain and Language, 120(3), 310320. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.
2011.10.006
Badcock, N. A., Holt, G., Holden, A., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2012).
dopOSCCI: A functional transcranial Doppler ultrasonogra-
phy summary suite for the assessment of cerebral lateraliza-
tion of cognitive function. Journal of Neuroscience Methods,
204(2), 383388. doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2011.11.018
Badcock, N. A., Nye, A., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2012). Using func-
tional transcranial Doppler ultrasonography to assess
language lateralisation: Influence of task and difficulty
level. Laterality, 17(6), 694710. doi:10.1080/1357650X.2011.
615128
Badcock, N. A., Pascoe, J. D., & Groen, M. A. (2016). How reliable
is the Pinaya method for assessing cognitive lateralization
with functional Transcranial Doppler ultrasound? Journal of
Medical Systems, 40(9), 205. doi:10.1007/s10916-016-0563-9
Badcock, N. A., Spooner, R., Hofmann, J., Flitton, A., Elliott, S.,
Kurylowicz, L., Keage, H. A. (2016). What Box: A task for
assessing language lateralisation in young children. PeerJ
PrePrints, 4(e1939v2). doi:10.7287/peerj.preprints.1939v2
Bakker, S. L. M., de Leeuw, F.-E., den Heijer, T., Koudstaal, P. J.,
Hofman, A., & Breteler, M. M. B. (2004). Cerebral
Haemodynamics in the elderly: The Rotterdam study.
Neuroepidemiology, 23(4), 178184. doi:10.1159/000078503
Bishop, D. V. M. (2013). Cerebral asymmetry and language
development: Cause, correlate, or consequence? Science,
340(6138), 1230531. doi:10.1126/science.1230531
Bishop, D. V. M., Badcock, N. A., & Holt, G. (2010). Assessment of
cerebral lateralization in children using Functional
Transcranial Doppler Ultrasound (fTCD). Journal of
Visualized Experiments, (43). doi:10.3791/2161
Bishop, D. V. M., Holt, G., Whitehouse, A. J. O., & Groen, M. (2014).
No population bias to left-hemisphere language in 4-year-
olds
with language impairment. PeerJ, 2, e507. doi:10.7717/
peerj.507
Bishop, D. V. M., Watt, H., & Papadatou-Pastou, M. (2009). An
efficient and reliable method for measuring cerebral laterali-
zation during speech with functional transcranial Doppler
ultrasound. Neuropsychologia, 47(2), 587590. doi:10.1016/j.
neuropsychologia.2008.09.013
Boatman, D., Hart, J., Lesser, R. P., Honeycutt, N., Anderson, N. B.,
Miglioretti, D., & Gordon, B. (1998). Right hemisphere speech
perception revealed by amobarbital injection and electrical
interference. Neurology, 51(2), 458464. doi:10.1212/WNL.
51.2.458
Buchinger, C., Flöel, A., Lohmann, H., Deppe, M., Henningsen, H.,
& Knecht, S. (2000). Lateralization of expressive and receptive
language functions in healthy volunteers. NeuroImage, 11(5),
S317. doi:10.1016/S1053-8119(00)91249-7
Carod Artal, F. J., Vazquez Cabrera, C. V., & Horan, T. A. (2004).
Lateralization of cerebral blood flow velocity changes
during auditory stimulation: A functional transcranial
Doppler Study. Applied Neuropsychology, 11(3), 167174.
doi:10.1207/s15324826an1103_5
Chilosi, A. M., Comparini, A., Cristofani, P., Turi, M., Berrettini, S.,
Forli, F., Cioni, G. (2014). Cerebral lateralization for
language in deaf children with cochlear implantation. Brain
and Language, 129,16. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2013.12.002
Deppe, M., Knecht, S., Henningsen, H., & Ring elstein, E. B.
(1997). AVERAGE: A Windows® program for aut omated
analysis of event related cerebral blood flow. Journal of
Neuroscience Methods, 75(2), 147154. doi:10.1016/S0165-
0270(97)00067-8
Deppe, M., Knecht, S., Lohmann, H., & Ringelstein, E. B. (2004). A
method for the automated assessment of temporal charac-
teristics of functional hemispheric lateralization by transcra-
nial Doppler sonography. Journal of Neuroimaging, 14(3),
226230. doi:10.1111/j.1552-6569.2004.tb00242.x
Deppe, M., Knecht, S., Papke, K., Lohmann, H., Fleischer, H.,
Heindel, W., Henningsen, H. (2000
). Assessment of hemi-
sp
heric language lateralization: A comparison between fMRI
and fTCD. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow & Metabolism, 20(2),
263268. doi:10.1097/00004647-200002000-00006
Deppe, M., Ringelstein, E. B., & Knecht, S. (2004). The investi-
gation of functional brain lateralization by transcranial
Doppler sonography. NeuroImage, 21(3), 11241146. doi:10.
1016/j.neuroimage.2003.10.016
DEsposito, M., Zarahn, E., & Aguirre, G. K. (1999). Event-related
functional MRI: implications for cognitive psychology.
Psychological Bulletin, 125(1), 155164.
Dräger, B., Jansen, A., Bruchmann, S., Förster, A. F., Pleger, B.,
Zwitserlood, P., & Knecht, S. (2004). How does the brain
accommodate to increased task difficulty in word finding?
A functional MRI study. NeuroImage, 23(3), 11521160.
doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.005
Dräger, B., & Knecht, S. (2002). When finding words becomes
difficult: Is there activation of the subdominant hemisphere?
NeuroImage, 16(3, Part A), 794800. doi:10.1006/nimg.2002.
1095
Droste, D. W., Harders, A. G., & Rastogi, E. (1989). A transcranial
Doppler study of blood flow velocity in the middle cerebral
arteries performed at rest and during mental activities.
Stroke, 20(8), 10051011. doi:10.1161/01.STR.20.8.1005
Escartin, C., & Rouach, N. (2013). Astroglial networking contributes
to neurometabolic coupling. Frontiers in Neuroenergetics, 5.
doi:10.3389/fnene.2013.00004
Gaillard, W. D., Hertz-Pannier, L., Mott, S. H., Barnett, A. S.,
LeBihan, D., & Theodore, W. H. (2000). Functional anatomy
of cognitive development fMRI of verbal fluency in children
and adults. Neurology, 54(1), 180180. doi:10.1212/WNL.54.1.
180
Gibo, H., Carver, C. C., Rhoton, A. L., Lenkey, C., & Mitchell, R. J.
(1981). Microsurgical anatomy of the middle cerebral
artery. Journal of Neurosurgery, 54(2), 151169. doi:10.3171/
jns.1981.54.2.0151
Groen, M. A., Whitehouse, A. J. O., Badcock, N. A., & Bishop,
D.
V. M. (2012). Does cerebral lateralization develop? A
study using functional transcranial Doppler ultrasound asses-
sing lateralization for language production and visuospatial
8 N. A. BADCOCK AND M. A. GROEN
memory. Brain and Behavior, 2(3), 256269. doi:10.1002/brb3.
56
Groen, M. A., Whitehouse, A. J. O., Badcock, N. A., & Bishop,
D. V. M. (2013). Associations between handedness and cer-
ebral lateralisation for language: A comparison of three
measures in children. PLoS ONE, 8(5), e64876. doi:10.1371/
journal.pone.0064876
de Guibert, C., Maumet, C., Jannin, P., Ferré, J.-C., Tréguier, C.,
Barillot, C., Biraben, A. (2011). Abnormal functional
lateralization and activity of language brain areas in typical
specific language impairment (developmental dysphasia).
Brain, 134(10), 30443058. doi:10.1093/brain/awr141
Gutierrez-Sigut, E., Daws, R., Payne, H., Blott, J., Marshall, C., &
MacSweeney, M. (2015). Language lateralization of hearing
native signers: A functional transcranial Doppler sonography
(fTCD) study of speech and sign production. Brain and
Language, 151,2334. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2015.10.006
Gutierrez-Sigut, E., Payne, H., & MacSweeney, M. (2015).
Investigating language lateralization during phonological
and semantic fluency tasks using functional transcranial
Doppler sonography. Laterality, 20(1), 4968. doi:10.1080/
1357650X.2014.914950
Haag, A., Moeller, N., Knake, S., Hermsen, A., Oertel, W. H.,
Rosenow, F., & Hamer, H. M. (2010). Language lateralization
in children using functional transcranial Doppler sonogra-
phy. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 52(4), 331
336. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2009.03362.x
Hartje,W.,Ringelstein,E.B.,Kistinger,B.,Fabianek,D.,&
Willmes, K. (1994). Transcranial Dop pler ultrasonic assess-
ment of middle cerebral artery blood flow velocity
changes during verbal and visuospatial cogni tive tasks.
Neuropsychologia, 32(12), 14431452. doi:10.1016/0028-
3932(94)90116-3
Hausmann, M., Slabbekoorn, D., Van Goozen, S. H., Cohen-
Kettenis, P. T., & Güntürkün, O. (2000). Sex hormones affect
spatial abilities during the menstrual cycle. Behavioral
Neuroscience, 114(6), 12451250. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.
114.6.1245
Helmstaedter, C., Jockwitz, C., & Witt, J.-A. (2015). Menstrual
cycle corrupts reliable and valid assessment of language
dominance: Consequences for presurgical evaluation of
patients with epilepsy. Seizure
, 28,2
631. doi:10.1016/j.
seizure.2015.02.010
Hirnstein, M., Westerhausen, R., Korsnes, M. S., & Hugdahl, K.
(2013). Sex differences in language asymmetry are age-
dependent and small: A large-scale, consonantvowel dicho-
tic listening study with behavioral and fMRI data. Cortex, 49
(7), 19101921. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2012.08.002
Hodgson, J. C., Hirst, R. J., & Hudson, J. M. (2016). Hemispheric
speech lateralisation in the developing brain is related to
motor praxis ability. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience,
22,917. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2016.09.005
Holland, S. K., Plante, E., Weber Byars, A., Strawsburg, R. H.,
Schmithorst, V. J., & Ball Jr., W. S. (2001). Normal fMRI brain
activation patterns in children performing a verb generation
task. NeuroImage, 14(4), 837843. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.
0875
Holland, S. K., Vannest, J., Mecoli, M., Jacola, L. M., Tillema, J.-M.,
Karunanayaka, P. R., Byars, A. W. (2007). Functional MRI of
language lateralization during development in children.
International Journal of Audiology, 46(9), 533551. doi:10.
1080/14992020701448994
Illingworth, S., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2009). Atypical cerebral later-
alisation in adults with compensated developmental dys-
lexia demonstrated using functional transcranial Doppler
ultrasound. Brain and Language, 111(1), 6165. doi:10.1016/
j.bandl.2009.05.002
Iova, A., Garmashov, A., Androuchtchenko, N., Koberidse, I., Berg,
D., & Garmashov, J. (2004). Evaluation of the ventricular
system in children using transcranial ultrasound: Reference
values for routine diagnostics. Ultrasound in Medicine &
Biology, 30(6), 745751. doi:10.1016/j.ultrasmedbio.2004.04.001
Kattan, K. R. (1970). Calvarial thickening after Dilantin medi-
cation. American Journal of Roentgenology, 110(1), 102105.
doi:10.2214/ajr.110.1.102
Keage, H. A. D., Churches, O. F., Kohler, M., Pomeroy, D.,
Luppino, R., Bartolo, M. L., & Elliott, S. (2012).
Cerebrovascular function in aging and dementia: A systema-
tic review of transcranial Doppler studies. Dementia
and
Geriatric Cognitive Disorders EXTRA, 2(1), 258270. doi:10.
1159/000339234
Keage, H. A. D., Kurylowicz, L., Lavrencic, L. M., Churches, O. F.,
Flitton, A., Hofmann, J., Badcock, N. A. (2015).
Cerebrovascular function associated with fluid, not crystal-
lized, abilities in older adults: A transcranial Doppler study.
Psychology and Aging, 30(3), 613623. doi:10.1037/
pag0000026
Knake, S., Haag, A., Hamer, H. M., Dittmer, C., Bien, S., Oertel, W.
H., & Rosenow, F. (2003). Language lateralization in patients
with temporal lobe epilepsy: A comparison of functional
transcranial Doppler sonography and the Wada test.
NeuroImage, 19(3), 12281232. doi:10.1016/S1053-8119
(03)00174-5
Knake, S., Haag, A., Pilgramm, G., Dittmer, C., Reis, J., Aßmann,
H., Hamer, H. M. (2006). Language dominance in mesial
temporal lobe epilepsy: A functional transcranial Doppler
sonography study of brain plasticity. Epilepsy & Behavior, 9
(2), 345348. doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2006.06.011
Knecht, S., Deppe, M., Ebner, A., Henningsen, H., Huber, T.,
Jokeit, H., & Ringelstein, E. B. (1998). Noninvasive determi-
nation of language lateralization by functional transcranial
Doppler sonography. A comparison with the Wada Test.
Stroke, 29(1), 8286. doi:10.1161/01.STR.29.1.82
Knecht, S., Deppe, M., Ringelstein, E. B., Wirtz, M., Lohmann, H.,
Dräger, B., Henningsen, H. (1998). Reproducibility of func-
tional transcranial Doppler sonography in determining hemi-
spheric language lateralization. Stroke, 29(6), 11551159.
doi:10.1161/01.STR.29.6.1155
Knecht, S., Dräger, B., Deppe, M., Bobe, L., Lohmann, H., Flöel, A.,
Henningsen, H. (2000). Handedness and hemispheric
language dominance in healthy humans. Brain, 123(12),
25122518. doi:10.1093/brain/123.12.2512
Knecht, S., Dräger, B., Flöel, A., Lohmann, H., Breitenstein, C.,
Deppe, M., Ringelstein, E.-B. (2001). Behavioural relevance
of atypical language lateralization in healthy subjects. Brain,
124(8), 1657
1665. doi:10.1093/brain/124.8.1657
Knecht,
S., Henningsen, H., Deppe, M., Huber, T., Ebner, A., &
Ringelstein, E. B. (1996). Successive activation of both cer-
ebral hemispheres during cued word generation.
Neuroreport, 7(3), 820824. doi:10.1097/00001756-
199602290-00033
Kohler, M., Keage, H. A. D., Spooner, R., Flitton, A., Hofmann, J.,
Churches, O. F., Badcock, N. A. (2015). Variability in latera-
lised blood flow response to language is associated with
LANGUAGE, COGNITION AN D NEUROSCIENCE 9
language development in children aged 15 years. Brain and
Language, 145146,3441. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2015.04.004
Lefebvre, E. B., Haining, R. G., & Labbé, R. F. (1972). Coarse facies,
calvarial thickening and hyperphosphatasia associated with
long-term anticonvulsant therapy. New England Journal of
Medicine, 286(24), 13011302. doi:10.1056/NEJM19720615
2862406
Li, B., & Freeman, R. D. (2015). Neurometabolic coupling
between neural activity, glucose, and lactate in activated
visual cortex. Journal of Neurochemistry, 135(4), 742754.
doi:10.1111/jnc.13143
Lohmann, H., Dräger, B., Müller-Ehrenberg, S., Deppe, M., &
Knecht, S. (2005). Language lateralization in young children
assessed by functional transcranial Doppler sonography.
NeuroImage, 24(3), 780790. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.
2004.08.053
Lohmann, H., Ringelstein, E. B., & Knecht, S. (2006). Functional
transcranial Doppler sonography. In R. W. Baumgartner
(Ed.), Frontiers of neurology and neuroscience (pp. 251260).
Basel: KARGER. Retrieved from http://www.karger.com/doi/
10.1159/000092437
Luck, S. J. (2014). An introduction to the event-related potential
technique. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Meyer, G. F., Spray, A., Fairlie, J. E., & Uomini, N. T. (2014).
Inferring common cognitive mechanisms from brain blood-
flow lateralization data: A new methodology for fTCD analy-
sis. Cognition, 5, 552. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00552
Miyazaki, M., & Kato, K. (1965). Measurement of cerebral blood
flow by ultrasonic Doppler technique: Theory. Japanese
Circulation Journal, 29(4), 375382. doi:10.1253/jcj.29.375
Payne, H., Gutierrez-Sigut, E., Subik, J., Woll, B., & MacSweeney,
M. (2015). Stimulus rate increases lateralisation in linguistic
and non-linguistic tasks measured by functional transcranial
Doppler sonography. Neuropsychologia, 72,5969. doi:10.
1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.04.019
Pelletier, I., Sauerwein, H. C., Lepore, F., Saint-Amour, D., &
Lassonde,
M. (2007). Non-invasive alternatives to the Wada
test in the presurgical evaluation of language and memory
functions in epilepsy patients. Epileptic Disorders:
International Epilepsy Journal with Videotape, 9(2), 111126.
doi:10.1684/epd.2007.0109
Pinaya, W. H. L., Fraga, F. J., Haratz, S. S., Dean, P. J. A., Conforto,
A. B., Bor-Seng-Shu, E., Sato, J. R. (2015). Comparing
methods for determining motor-hand lateralization based
on fTCD signals. Journal of Medical Systems, 39(2), 19.
doi:10.1007/s10916-014-0185-z
Price, C. J. (2010). The anatomy of language: A review of 100
fMRI studies published in 2009. Annals of the New York
Academy of Sciences, 1191(1), 6288. doi:10.1111/j.1749-
6632.2010.05444.x
Rihs, F., Gutbrod, K., Gutbrod, B., Steiger, H.-J., Sturzenegger,
M.,&Mattle,H.P.(1995). Determination of cognitive
hemispheric dominance by Stereo transcranial Doppler
sonography. Stroke, 26(1), 7073 . doi:10.1161/01.STR.26.1.
70
Ringelstein, E. B., Kahlscheuer, B., Niggemeyer, E., & Otis, S. M.
(1990). Transcranial Doppler sonography: Anatomical land-
marks and normal velocity values. Ultrasound in Medicine &
Biology, 16(8), 745761. doi:10.1016/0301-5629(90)90039-F
Rogers, L. J., & Vallortigara, G. (2015). When and why did brains
break symmetry? Symmetry, 7(4), 21812194. doi:10.3390/
sym7042181
Rosch, R. E., Bishop, D. V. M., & Badcock, N. A. (2012). Lateralised
visual attention is unrelated to language lateralisation, and
not influenced by task difficulty A functional transcranial
Doppler study. Neuropsychologia, 50(5), 810815. doi:10.
1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.01.015
Rosengarten, B., Osthaus, S., & Kaps, M. (2002
). Overshoot and
undershoot:
Control system analysis of haemodynamics in
a functional transcranial Doppler test. Cerebrovascular
Diseases, 14(34), 148152. doi:10.1159/000065672
Sarkar, S., Ghosh, S., Ghosh, S. K., & Collier, A. (2007). Role of tran-
scranial Doppler ultrasonography in stroke. Postgraduate
Medical Journal, 83(985), 683689. doi:10.1136/pgmj.2007.
058602
Satomura, S., & Kaneko, Z. (1960). Ultrasonic blood rheograph.
Proceedings of 3rd International Conference on Medical
Electronics, 254258.
Schultz, N. B., Matthews, G., Warm, J. S., & Washburn, D. A.
(2009). A transcranial Doppler sonography study of shoot/
dont-shoot responding. Behavior Research Methods, 41(3),
593597. doi:10.3758/BRM.41.3.593
Silvestrini, M., Cupini, L. M., Matteis, M., Troisi, E., & Caltagirone,
C. (1994). Bilateral simultaneous assessment of cerebral flow
velocity during mental activity. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow
& Metabolism, 14(4), 643648. doi:10.1038/jcbfm.1994.80
Somers, M., Neggers, S. F. W., Diederen, K. M., Boks, M. P., Kahn,
R. S., & Sommer, I. E. (2011). The measurement of language
lateralization with functional transcranial Doppler and func-
tional MRI: A critical evaluation. Frontiers in Human
Neuroscience, 5, 31. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2011.00031
Somers, M., Ophoff, R. A., Aukes, M. F., Cantor, R. M., Boks, M. P.,
Dauwan, M., Sommer, I. E. (2015). Linkage analysis in a
Dutch population isolate shows no major gene for left-hand-
edness or atypical language lateralization. The Journal of
Neuroscience, 35(23), 87308736. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.
3287-14.2015
Sommer, I. E., Aleman, A., Bouma, A., & Kahn, R. S. (2004). Do
women really have more bilateral language representation
than men? A meta-analysis of functional imaging studies.
Brain, 127(8), 18451852. doi:10.1093/brain/awh207
Sommer, I. E., Aleman, A., Somers, M., Boks, M. P., & Kahn, R. S.
(2008). Sex differences in handedness, asymmetry of the
Planum Temporale and functional language lateralization.
Br
ain Research, 1206, 7688. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2008.01.003
Stroobant, N., Buijs, D., & Vingerhoets, G. (2009). Variation in
brain lateralization during various language tasks: A func-
tional transcranial Doppler study. Behavioural Brain
Research, 199(2), 190196. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2008.11.040
Stroobant, N., Van Boxstael, J., & Vingerhoets, G. (2011).
Language lateralization in children: A functional transcranial
Doppler reliability study. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 24(1),
1424. doi:10.1016/j.jneuroling.2010.07.003
Sun, Y.-F., Lee, J.-S., & Kirby, R. (2010). Brain imaging findings in
Dyslexia. Pediatrics & Neonatology, 51(2), 8996. doi:10.1016/
S1875-9572(10)60017-4
Szaflarski, J. P., Holland, S. K., Schmithorst, V. J., & Byars, A. W.
(2006). An fMRI study of language lateralization in children
and adults. Human Brain Mapping, 27(3), 202212. doi:10.
1002/hbm.20177
Szaflarski, J. P., Schmithorst, V. J., Altaye, M., Byars, A. W., Ret, J.,
Plante, E., & Holland, S. K. (2006). A longitudinal fMRI study of
language development in children age 511. Annals of
Neurology, 59(5), 796807. doi:10.1002/ana.20817
10 N. A. B ADCOCK AND M. A. GROEN
Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Josse, G., Crivello, F., & Mazoyer, B. (2004).
Interindividual variability in the hemispheric organization for
speech. NeuroImage, 21(1), 422435. doi:10.1016/j.neuro
image.2003.08.032
Vallortigara, G., & Rogers, L. J. (2005). Survival with an asymme-
trical brain: Advantages and disadvantages of cerebral later-
alization. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(4), 575589.
doi:10.1017/S0140525X05000105
Villringer, A., & Dirnagl, U. (1995). Coupling of brain activity and
cerebral blood flow: Basis of functional neuroimaging.
Cerebrovascular and Brain Metabolism Reviews, 7(3), 240276.
Walsh, V., & Cowey, A. (2000). Transcranial magnetic stimulation
and cognitive neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience,
1(1), 7380. doi:10.1038/35036239
Whitehouse, A. J. O., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2009). Hemispheric div-
ision of function is the result of independent probabilistic
biases. Neuropsychologia, 47(89), 19381943. doi:10.1016/j.
neuropsychologia.2009.03.005
Wijnhoud, A. D., Franckena, M., van der Lugt, A., Koudstaal, P. J.,
& Dippel, D. W. J. (2008). Inadequate acoustical temporal
bone window in patients with a transient ischemic attack
or minor stroke: Role of skull thickness and bone density.
Ultrasound in Medicine & Biology, 34(6), 923929. doi:10.
1016/j.ultrasmedbio.2007.11.022
van der Zwan, A., Hillen, B., Tulleken, C. A., & Dujovny, M. (1993).
A quantitative investigation of the variability of the major
cerebral arterial territories. Stroke, 24(12), 19511959.
doi:10.1161/01.STR.24.12.1951
LANGUAGE, COGNITION AND NEUROSCIENCE 11